2004
DOI: 10.1123/mcj.8.1.76
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Control of Memory-Guided Reaching Movements in Peripersonal Space

Abstract: The goal of the present investigation was to explore the putative contributions of feedforward- and feedback-based processes in the control of memory-guided reaching movements. Participants (N = 4) completed an extensive number of reaching movements (2700) to 3 midline targets (20, 30, 40 cm) in 6 visual conditions: full-vision, open-loop, and four memory-guided conditions (0, 200, 400, and 600 ms of delay). To infer limb control, we used a regression technique to examine the within-trial correspondence betwee… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
64
2

Year Published

2004
2004
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 90 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
4
64
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Quick or slow goal-directed movements to visually remembered target locations often result in different endpoint locations when compared to reaching to veridical targets (Darling and Miller 1993;Westwood et al 2001;Heath and Westwood 2003;Heath et al 2004;Hondzinski and Cui 2006;Hondzinski and Kwon 2009). Relatively large endpoint errors can occur when the movement to a remembered target location is performed without vision of the hand (Prablanc et al 1986;Admiraal et al 2003;Heath and Westwood 2003;Admiraal et al 2004;Heath 2005;Sarlegna and Blouin 2010) or when inaccurate, indiscernible visual feedback of hand position is provided during the movement (Sarlegna et al 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Quick or slow goal-directed movements to visually remembered target locations often result in different endpoint locations when compared to reaching to veridical targets (Darling and Miller 1993;Westwood et al 2001;Heath and Westwood 2003;Heath et al 2004;Hondzinski and Cui 2006;Hondzinski and Kwon 2009). Relatively large endpoint errors can occur when the movement to a remembered target location is performed without vision of the hand (Prablanc et al 1986;Admiraal et al 2003;Heath and Westwood 2003;Admiraal et al 2004;Heath 2005;Sarlegna and Blouin 2010) or when inaccurate, indiscernible visual feedback of hand position is provided during the movement (Sarlegna et al 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Elliott et al, 1991), although the mean difference between vision conditions was in the expected direction for TAPV (i.e., FV = 251 ms vs. NV = 243 ms). Notably, other studies have also failed to detect TAPV differences between FV and NV conditions (e.g., Heath et al, 2004).…”
Section: Conventional Measures Of Online Feedback Utilizationmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Building on the Elliott et al (1999) procedure, Heath, Westwood, and Binsted (2004) calculated cumulative amplitudes at a number of kinematic markers (i.e., PA, PV, PD) and then separately correlated these amplitudes with the amplitude achieved at the end of the movement. Because the amplitudes were cumulative, early amplitude error was posited to predict late amplitude error if there was limited online regulation of the movement based on feedback (i.e., a robust r 2 value).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although Heath and colleagues have shown reliable vision-no-vision differences using this method, the main thrust of much of their work has been to identify differences in limb control associated with memory-driven movements. In their protocols, they have also employed multiple target amplitudes (Heath et al, 2004) and conditions in which the target remained illuminated while the limb was occluded (Heath, 2005). In this context, they have found that even very brief no-vision periods prior to movement initiation result in decreased online control.…”
Section: Discontinuities In the Trajectory And Their Effectivenessmentioning
confidence: 99%