2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The cognitive mechanisms underlying perspective taking between conversational partners: Evidence from speakers with Alzheimer׳s disease

Abstract: Successful communication requires speakers to consider their listeners’ perspectives. Little is known about how this ability changes in Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) although such knowledge could reveal the cognitive mechanisms fundamental to perspective-taking ability, and reveal which cognitive deficits are fundamental to communication disorders in AD. Patients with mild to moderate AD and age and education matched controls were tested in a communicative perspective-taking task, and on measures of executive contr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
19
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
1
19
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Conditions included a “common ground” condition, where the participant and the avatar had equal access to all of the objects in a scene; a “colorblind” condition, where the participant was told that the avatar is only able to see in grayscale; and a “privileged ground” condition, where an opaque backing to the shelf physically blocked the avatar from seeing some of the objects on the shelf. The common ground and privileged ground conditions are comparable to those used in previous research on visual perspective-taking, including seminal work by Keysar and colleagues (Keysar, Barr, Balin, & Brauner, 2000; Keysar, Lin, & Barr, 2003) and follow-up studies by Wardlow, Brown-Schmidt, and others (Brown-Schmidt, 2009; Wardlow, 2013; Wardlow, Ivanova, & Gollan, 2014). However, these studies generally use smaller arrays of objects that may not adequately mimic the complex and demanding nature of real-world communication.…”
mentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Conditions included a “common ground” condition, where the participant and the avatar had equal access to all of the objects in a scene; a “colorblind” condition, where the participant was told that the avatar is only able to see in grayscale; and a “privileged ground” condition, where an opaque backing to the shelf physically blocked the avatar from seeing some of the objects on the shelf. The common ground and privileged ground conditions are comparable to those used in previous research on visual perspective-taking, including seminal work by Keysar and colleagues (Keysar, Barr, Balin, & Brauner, 2000; Keysar, Lin, & Barr, 2003) and follow-up studies by Wardlow, Brown-Schmidt, and others (Brown-Schmidt, 2009; Wardlow, 2013; Wardlow, Ivanova, & Gollan, 2014). However, these studies generally use smaller arrays of objects that may not adequately mimic the complex and demanding nature of real-world communication.…”
mentioning
confidence: 55%
“…Only a limited number of studies to date have examined perspective-taking in a communicative context (Dumontheil et al, 2010; Hillebrandt et al, 2013; Wardlow, 2013; Wardlow et al, 2014). For example, in an fMRI study of healthy volunteers, Dumontheil et al, (2010) used a variant of the director task, originally described by Keysar and colleagues (Keysar et al, 2000, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The paradigm in both studies, however, is comprehension-based and requires subjects to identify the target object via a motor response. Some referential communication studies in healthy adults and patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Wardlow, 2013; Wardlow et al, 2014) do involve overt speech responses, but not embedded within a discourse context. These studies also used smaller arrays of objects that may not adequately mimic the complex nature of real-world conversation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In children, inhibitory control is negatively correlated with communicative egocentrism (Nilsen & Graham, 2009). At the other end of the lifespan, Wardlow, Ivanova, and Gollan (2014) Here, we explore the simultaneous contributions of inhibition and switching to performance in a conversational perspective--taking task. Interestingly, these EF capacities are associated with two semi--independent (yet possibly concurrently engaged) modes of cognitive control.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%