2015
DOI: 10.1111/tops.12165
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Cognitive Advantages of Counting Specifically: A Representational Analysis of Verbal Numeration Systems in Oceanic Languages

Abstract: The domain of numbers provides a paradigmatic case for investigating interactions of culture, language, and cognition: Numerical competencies are considered a core domain of knowledge, and yet the development of specifically human abilities presupposes cultural and linguistic input by way of counting sequences. These sequences constitute systems with distinct structural properties, the cross-linguistic variability of which has implications for number representation and processing. Such representational effects… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
26
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
26
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Bender, S. Beller / Cognitive Science 41 (2017) numeme: X = rogo'uru (10), C = rau (100), K = takau (10), P = paua (20), T = tataua (40), and V = varu (80). It should be noted here that this is not (reliably) so in English, which is irregular in this regard: "ten" and "two" in English yields not "ten-two," but "twelve" (for implications of this irregularity, see, for example, Beller & Bender, 2011;Bender et al, 2015). 2 Additions (and multiplications) with 10 (X) are part of the classical addition (and multiplication) tables and are therefore also integrated in Table 2; the same holds for addition with 100 (C) in Table 3A and for addition with 80 (V) in Table 3B.…”
Section: Basic Addition Factsmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Bender, S. Beller / Cognitive Science 41 (2017) numeme: X = rogo'uru (10), C = rau (100), K = takau (10), P = paua (20), T = tataua (40), and V = varu (80). It should be noted here that this is not (reliably) so in English, which is irregular in this regard: "ten" and "two" in English yields not "ten-two," but "twelve" (for implications of this irregularity, see, for example, Beller & Bender, 2011;Bender et al, 2015). 2 Additions (and multiplications) with 10 (X) are part of the classical addition (and multiplication) tables and are therefore also integrated in Table 2; the same holds for addition with 100 (C) in Table 3A and for addition with 80 (V) in Table 3B.…”
Section: Basic Addition Factsmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Nonetheless, in any case, none of these strategies gets by with fewer than three stepsand this is substantially more than the transformations required for the mixed system (for a more detailed account of the role that memory access may play for these steps, see Bender et al, 2015). S10 thus requires three distinct steps (if we disregard joining), and the same is true for A10 (v.ii).…”
Section: Addition Factsmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The construal of numeration systems, for instance, is generally based on a limited set of key properties, yet the concrete instantiations of these properties and their combination may vary substantially even in related languages—numbers are represented in a language‐specific format. These differences in representational format, in turn, can be assumed to affect the processing of numerical information, even if only as a matter of degree rather than kind, for instance, by facilitating some types of algorithmic processes relative to other representational formats (Bender, Schlimm, & Beller, ). Likewise, the subject of body part taxonomies is naturally the same for all mankind, and both the visual and embodied perception of the human body set strong constraints on which discontinuities should be regarded as relevant in principle.…”
Section: Cognitive Diversitymentioning
confidence: 99%