2018
DOI: 10.1037/dev0000417
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The codevelopment of sympathy and overt aggression from middle childhood to early adolescence.

Abstract: We assessed the extent to which feelings of sympathy and aggressive behaviors co-developed from 6 to 12 years of age in a representative sample of Swiss children (N = 1,273). Caregivers and teachers reported children's sympathy and overt aggression in three-year intervals. Secondorder latent curve models indicated general mean-level declines in sympathy and overt aggression over time, although the decline in sympathy was relatively small. Importantly, both trajectories were characterized by significant inter-i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
23
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
2
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the present study, we focused on physical aggression (e.g., attacking others and causing bodily harm) for the following reasons: (1) it is thought to be particularly contingent on developmentally invariant factors, such as resting HR, whereas other aggressive and rule‐breaking behaviors are more susceptible to contextual factors (e.g., peer pressure–induced exclusive behavior; Eisner & Malti, ); (2) its consequences are relatively severe and visible to children, thus increasing its likelihood of triggering guilt and sympathetic concern (Zuffianò, Colasante, Buchmann, & Malti, ); and (3) it occurs—albeit to different extents—in early, middle, and late childhood (Tremblay, ), thus spanning the age groups under study.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the present study, we focused on physical aggression (e.g., attacking others and causing bodily harm) for the following reasons: (1) it is thought to be particularly contingent on developmentally invariant factors, such as resting HR, whereas other aggressive and rule‐breaking behaviors are more susceptible to contextual factors (e.g., peer pressure–induced exclusive behavior; Eisner & Malti, ); (2) its consequences are relatively severe and visible to children, thus increasing its likelihood of triggering guilt and sympathetic concern (Zuffianò, Colasante, Buchmann, & Malti, ); and (3) it occurs—albeit to different extents—in early, middle, and late childhood (Tremblay, ), thus spanning the age groups under study.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For illustrative purposes of the STMS model, we analyzed data published in Zuffianò et al [10]. Data were from a cohort of 6year-olds (reassessed at ages 9 and 12) from the Swiss Survey…”
Section: Methods Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sympathy is a specific emotional response that includes feelings of concern or sorrow for another's emotional state or welfare [5]. In comparison to empathy, which generally involves sharing the emotions of another, but not necessarily feeling concern for them, sympathy is more likely to be implicated in prosocial and aggressive behaviors [9,10].…”
Section: Cross-informant Convergence In the Assessment Of Children's mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Aggression has been found to be related negatively to sympathy from the age of approximately 6 years onwards (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Morris, 2014;Zuffianò, Colasante, Buchmann, & Malti, 2018). Yet, during the preschool years, there is evidence for a positive relation between aggressiveness and prosocial tendencies (Feshbach & Feshbach, 1969;Gill & Calkins, 2003;Kienbaum, 2001;Radke-Yarrow & Zahn-Waxler, 1976).…”
Section: Child Personalitymentioning
confidence: 99%