1987
DOI: 10.1177/014272378700701903
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The child's knowledge of English kin terms

Abstract: Five groups of subjects (three-, four-, five-, and six-year-olds as well as adults) with four males and four females per group were interviewed and questioned with respect to their knowledge of twelve English kin terms: mother, father, sister, brother, daughter, son, wife, husband, aunt, uncle, grandmother, and grandfather. A questionnaire, which in the case of the children was filled out by their parents, helped determine rank orderings for the twelve terms with respect to the estimated relative frequency sub… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
24
1

Year Published

1992
1992
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
(65 reference statements)
2
24
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Although previous studies have shown that 3-and 4-year-old children understand kinship terms (Benson & Anglin, 1987) and act more prosocially toward kin than toward nonkin in daily life (de Guzman, Carlo, & Edwards, 2008), children aged 3 and 4 years who were able to match a kinship term with a particular person did not exhibit kin altruism when allocating resources in the current experimental setting. In daily life, children may share more with kin because they are asked to do so by a particular relative such as a parent or sibling.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although previous studies have shown that 3-and 4-year-old children understand kinship terms (Benson & Anglin, 1987) and act more prosocially toward kin than toward nonkin in daily life (de Guzman, Carlo, & Edwards, 2008), children aged 3 and 4 years who were able to match a kinship term with a particular person did not exhibit kin altruism when allocating resources in the current experimental setting. In daily life, children may share more with kin because they are asked to do so by a particular relative such as a parent or sibling.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…Some studies have focused on kinship concepts, showing that children as young as 3 years were able to understand kinship terms (Benson & Anglin, 1987) and that 5-year-olds could explain kin relationships (Benson & Anglin, 1987;Macaskill, 1981). Other studies have investigated how preschoolers came to understand facial resemblance in cuing kinship relationships (Kaminski, Gentaz, & Mazens, 2012) and how young children identified parent-child dyads with similar physical characteristics such as height and skin color and with similar personality traits such as generosity (Springer, 1996;Williams & Smith, 2010).…”
Section: Kin Altruism-related Research In Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the literature on semantic and conceptual development, adulthood is frequently viewed as the developmental end-point where representations of nominal kinds are overwhelmingly organized around defining features (Benson & Anglin, 1987;Keil, 1986;Keil & Batterman, 1984). However, the Yoruba adults in all three groups in the present study did not show anywhere near exclusive use of defining features for most of the concepts.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The dialectical relationship between the external world and conceptual structure provides an hypothesis. Many researchers agree that natural concepts reflect something of the sociocultural contexts in which they are embedded (Carey, 1985;Keil, 1986;Murphy & Medin, 1985;Seiler & Wannenmacher, 1983). Since neither the individual nor the contexts of which he or she is a part are rigid and unchanging, it follows that any dramatic change in either will be reflected in the pertinent concepts.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation