1995
DOI: 10.1007/bf01499375
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The believability of repressed memories.

Abstract: Two experiments investigated how mock jurors react to a case involving a repressed memory of child sexual assault. Subjects read a fictional civil trial (Experiment 1) or criminal trial (Experiment 2) summary involving the sexual assault of a 6-year-old female. The summary was presented in one of three conditions: (a) child condition: the alleged victim reported her memory of the assault in the same year that the assault occurred; (b) repressed condition: the alleged victim reported the assault 20 years later,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
78
3

Year Published

1999
1999
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(98 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
17
78
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This gender difference is consistent with previous literature showing that female mock jurors find sexual abuse victims more credible than male mock jurors and are more likely to convict (e.g., Golding, Sego, Sanchez, & Hasemann, 1995;McCauley & Parker, 2001). It is also consistent with some literature examining perceptions of physical abuse (Kean & Dukes, 1991).…”
Section: Participant Characteristicssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…This gender difference is consistent with previous literature showing that female mock jurors find sexual abuse victims more credible than male mock jurors and are more likely to convict (e.g., Golding, Sego, Sanchez, & Hasemann, 1995;McCauley & Parker, 2001). It is also consistent with some literature examining perceptions of physical abuse (Kean & Dukes, 1991).…”
Section: Participant Characteristicssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…In brief, research that has examined adult rape cases with a female victim and a male perpetrator indicates that men tend to blame victims more, identify and empathize with victims less, and attribute greater responsibility to victims (Deitz, Blackwell, Daley, & Bentley, 1982;Foley & Pigott, 2000;see Brekke & Borgida, 1988, for a review). A similar pattern of results is noted in the literature for child victims of sexual violence, in that female jurors, compared to male jurors, tend to make more pro-victim case decisions with respect to credibility, responsibility, or guilt judgments (Bottoms, 1993;Bottoms, Davis, & Epstein, 2004;Bottoms & Goodman, 1994;Crowley, O'Gallaghan, & Ball, 1994;Golding, Sanchez, & Sego, 1997Golding, Sego, Sanchez, & Hasemann, 1995;McCauley & Parker, 2001;Quas, Goodman, & Jones, 2003;Schmidt & Brigham, 1996; for a meta-analytic review, see Schutte & Hosch, 1997). Whether such a gender effect also would be observed in an SVP mock jury paradigm with a child victim has never been tested.…”
supporting
confidence: 51%
“…Previous studies have found mixed results as to whether continuous memories are perceived as more credible than recovered memories (Golding, Sego, Sanchez, & Hasemann, 1995, no significant differences; Loftus, Weingardt, & Hoffman, 1993, recovered memories < continuous memories Experiments 1 & 2), but what is clear from an inspection of the means is that recovered memories are at least moderately supported (Golding et al, 1995;Golding, Sego, & Sanchez, 1996;Loftus et al, 1993). The current study found both types of memories to be similarly perceived, with moderate to moderately high levels of credibility, although the means were higher in the naive memory recovery technique condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%