The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition 2017
DOI: 10.4324/9781315802541-12
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Acquisition of American Sign Language

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
56
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 197 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
3
56
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…As the majority of research on infants’ and young children’s interpretation of movement has been within the framework of external goals and actions on objects (e.g., Woodward, 1998), we know little about when young children interpret a movement as a representation (but see Novack, Goldin-Meadow, & Woodward, 2015). Although it is clear that young children exposed to a conventional sign language like ASL can learn that language as naturally, and following approximately the same timetable, as children exposed to a spoken language (Lillo-Martin, 1999; Newport & Meier, 1985), the as-yet-unanswered questions are—when do children interpret movements that are not part of a linguistic system as representations, and are the same cues to representation that we have found to be important to adults also important to children?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As the majority of research on infants’ and young children’s interpretation of movement has been within the framework of external goals and actions on objects (e.g., Woodward, 1998), we know little about when young children interpret a movement as a representation (but see Novack, Goldin-Meadow, & Woodward, 2015). Although it is clear that young children exposed to a conventional sign language like ASL can learn that language as naturally, and following approximately the same timetable, as children exposed to a spoken language (Lillo-Martin, 1999; Newport & Meier, 1985), the as-yet-unanswered questions are—when do children interpret movements that are not part of a linguistic system as representations, and are the same cues to representation that we have found to be important to adults also important to children?…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This previous work shows that classifier predicates are learned between the ages of 3;0 and 9;0 (years; months), morpheme by morpheme (rather than as unsegmented wholes; Ellenberger & Steyaert 1978;Newport 1981;Supalla 1982;Newport & Meier 1985;Lillo-Martin 1999;2009;Singleton & Newport 2004). The specific morphosyntactic system associated with the agentive/non-agentive opposition addressed here first appears between ages 4 and 6 years, but is not fully mastered until ages 7;0-10;0 in ASL (Schick 1987, Brentari et al 2013).…”
Section: Study 2: Child Signers and Gesturers In Italy The Us And Nmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…All studies to-date have focused on pronouns in spoken languages. In recent decades, a rapidly growing body of work has examined the signed languages of the deaf, which are full-fledged linguistic systems characterized by the hallmarks of human languages: for example, they are acquired naturally from birth by children exposed to them (Newport & Meier, 1985), they exhibit duality of patterning and syntactic recursion (Meier, 2002), and late-learners show critical period effects (Mayberry & Eichen, 1991; Mayberry, Lock, & Kazmi, 2002). A study of the use of sign language pronouns by TD and ASD deaf children could shed new light on the phenomena of pronoun reversal and avoidance.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%