1997
DOI: 10.1016/s0163-6383(97)90009-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The acoustic basis of preferences for infant-directed singing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

12
156
1
4

Year Published

2001
2001
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 183 publications
(173 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
12
156
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…In our opinion, this argument is more consistent with the results observed in the present three experiments since the patterns are easier to connect to findings in the vocal expression of emotion (Scherer et al, 2003;Juslin & Laukka, 2003), and even to infant-directed singing, than to the conventions of music. For example, infantdirected singing has relatively more energy at lower frequencies (Trainor, Clark, Huntley, & Adams, 1997), rendering it more pleasant to the listener (infant and adult), which is consistent with the observations of the present study concerning the Valence dimension. Also, the few studies dealing with aspects of timbre in communicating emotions in music (Goydke et al, 2004;Juslin, 2000;Scherer & Oshinsky, 1977) have drawn mutually consistent observations about the role of high-frequency energy content and articulation type relating to different emotional expressions.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…In our opinion, this argument is more consistent with the results observed in the present three experiments since the patterns are easier to connect to findings in the vocal expression of emotion (Scherer et al, 2003;Juslin & Laukka, 2003), and even to infant-directed singing, than to the conventions of music. For example, infantdirected singing has relatively more energy at lower frequencies (Trainor, Clark, Huntley, & Adams, 1997), rendering it more pleasant to the listener (infant and adult), which is consistent with the observations of the present study concerning the Valence dimension. Also, the few studies dealing with aspects of timbre in communicating emotions in music (Goydke et al, 2004;Juslin, 2000;Scherer & Oshinsky, 1977) have drawn mutually consistent observations about the role of high-frequency energy content and articulation type relating to different emotional expressions.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Perception of tempo is essential for communication and social interaction, as it conveys information about affect, intent, emphasis, and meaning (e.g., Bullowa, 1979;Jaffee, Beebe, Feldstein, Crown, & Jasnow, 2001;Stern, 1985;Trevarthan, 1993;WalkerAndrews, 1997). Infant-directed communication differs from adult-directed communication along a number of temporal dimensions, including tempo and rhythm, and infants discriminate and attend selectively to infant-directed speech, singing, and signing (e.g., Cooper & Aslin, 1990;Fernald, 1985;Jaffee, et al, 2001;Koester, Papousek, & Papousek, 1989;Masatka, 1996;Trainor, Clark, Huntley, & Adams, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An emotion hypothesis may also explain why ID speech is universally higher in pitch than is AD. There appears to be an intrinsic mapping between pitch and emotion (Scherer, 1986;Trainor et al, 2000;Trainor, Clark, Huntley, & Adams, 1997). Although the expression of fear can also be associated with high pitch in conjunction with a tense timbre (Scherer, 1986), across human cultures as well as across many animal species, low pitch tends to signal aggression, and high pitch tends to signal friendliness and nonaggression (Morton, 1977).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%