2001
DOI: 10.1348/026151001165985
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The ability of children with imaginary companions to differentiate between fantasy and reality

Abstract: This study investigated the ability of children with and without imaginary companions (N = 80) aged 4‐8 years to differentiate fantasy from reality. Following children's descriptions of a monster, a monster‐shaped silhouette was unexpectedly projected in the room. Video‐recordings of each child's reaction to the silhouette and responses to a subsequent series of questions indicated that a significantly larger number of imaginary companion than non‐imaginary companion children thought that an imaginary entity c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
0
3

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
(37 reference statements)
2
19
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The difference in our findings is most likely due to the greater sample size in this study. This finding contributes to a growing body of research that indicates that individual differences in fantasy orientation do not imply differences in overall ability to make the fantasy-reality distinction (Bouldin & Pratt, 2001;Taylor et al, 1993).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 51%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The difference in our findings is most likely due to the greater sample size in this study. This finding contributes to a growing body of research that indicates that individual differences in fantasy orientation do not imply differences in overall ability to make the fantasy-reality distinction (Bouldin & Pratt, 2001;Taylor et al, 1993).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 51%
“…One possibility is that children who are frequently immersed in fantasy and imaginative play are more likely to confuse their imagined representations for representations of real events. Bouldin and Pratt (2001) found that children with imaginary companions were more likely than other children to believe that an imagined monster had materialized inside a tent, but other studies have found no relationship or a negative relationship between fantasy orientation and fantasy beliefs (Dierker & Sanders, 1996/1997Prentice et al, 1978;Taylor, Cartwright, & Carlson, 1993;cf. Sharon & Woolley, 2004;Singer & Singer, 1981).…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Over the last couple of decades, children's ability to distinguish between pretense and reality has been examined (e.g., Bouldin & Pratt, 2001;Bourchier & Davis, 2002;Davis, Woolley, & Bruell, 2002). The old notion that children have poor reality testing has been challenged, and children's relations with pretend companions are now construed as a vivid merging point between fantasy and reality.…”
Section: Renewal Of Empirical Attentionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dissociations between verbal and behavioral responses have been obtained in a wide range of domains. Studies on children's ability to make the fantasy -reality distinction reveal mature responding when children are asked questions about fantasy, yet their behavior appears to reveal fantastical beliefs (Bouldin & Pratt, 2001;Harris, Brown, Marriot, Whittall, & Harmer, 1991;Johnson & Harris, 1994;Subbotsky, 1993). Other work similarly reveals more mature responding in verbalizations.…”
Section: Dissociations In Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of studies of children's magical and fantastical beliefs (Bouldin & Pratt, 2001;Harris et al, 1991;Johnson & Harris, 1994;Subbotsky, 1993) have revealed more mature knowledge in verbalizations than in behavior. In Harris et al (1991, Experiment 4), 4-to 6-year-old children looked into an empty box and pretended that either a monster or a rabbit was inside.…”
Section: Fantasy -Reality Distinction and Magical Thinkingmentioning
confidence: 99%