1985
DOI: 10.1016/s0163-6383(85)80017-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The 5-month-old's ability to discriminate facial expressions of emotion

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

5
87
0
2

Year Published

1996
1996
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 109 publications
(96 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
5
87
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…LeDoux (1996), for example, argues for a direct neural pathway from the sensory thalamus to the amygdala, which is able to support rapid and defensive responses to potentially dangerous stimuli, even before conscious identification and evaluation of the stimuli. Developmental research has also demonstrated that infants are efficient at noticing and recognizing negative facial expressions (e.g., Sackett, 1966;Schwartz et al, 1985). Furthermore, on the basis of investigations of monkeys reared in isolation, Sackett claims that pictures of animals of the same species displaying threat "appear to have prepotent general activating properties .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…LeDoux (1996), for example, argues for a direct neural pathway from the sensory thalamus to the amygdala, which is able to support rapid and defensive responses to potentially dangerous stimuli, even before conscious identification and evaluation of the stimuli. Developmental research has also demonstrated that infants are efficient at noticing and recognizing negative facial expressions (e.g., Sackett, 1966;Schwartz et al, 1985). Furthermore, on the basis of investigations of monkeys reared in isolation, Sackett claims that pictures of animals of the same species displaying threat "appear to have prepotent general activating properties .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A considerable amount of research has established that the visual system is highly efficient at perceiving both faces and facial expressions (e.g., Bruce, Desimone, & Gross, 1981;Desimone, 1991;Farah, Wilson, Drain, & Tanaka, 1998;Gorea & Julesz, 1990;Hasselmo, Rolls, & Baylis, 1989;Hochberg & Galper, 1967;Homa, Haver, & Schwartz, 1976;Purcell & Stewart, 1988;Schwartz, Izard, & Ansul, 1985;Tanaka & Farah, 1993). In fact, it has been suggested that the perception of facial expressions occurs automatically (e.g., Hansen & Hansen, 1994;Stenberg, Wilking, & Dahl, 1998) and without conscious awareness (e.g., Morris, Öhman, & Dolan, 1998).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children and adults show faster visual search for snakes and spiders than for harmless objects (LoBue, 2005;Ohman, Lundqvist, & Esteves, 2001). Young infants devote greater attention to angry faces than to faces depicting other emotions (Schwartz, Izard, & Ansul, 1985), and children and adults show more efficient visual search for angry faces than for neutral, happy, sad, or fearful faces (Fox, Lester, Russo, Bowles, Pichler & Dutton, 2000;Hansen & Hansen, 1988;LoBue, 2005;Williams & Mattingley, 2006). These findings demonstrate that children and adults alike attend to threat that is visible in a stimulus -such as an obviously harmful animal or apparently angry individual.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This obligatory reaction would lead infants to continue to look longer at the fearful expression than at a happy one even after habituation to the fearful expression. Similarly, infants may not show recovery FACIAL EXPRESSIONS O F EMOTION 579 of looking to angry expressions following habituation to other expressions because angry expressions induce gaze aversion (Schwartz et al, 1985).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%