1989
DOI: 10.3758/bf03208032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Texture perception in sighted and blind observers

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

10
94
1
4

Year Published

1991
1991
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 172 publications
(117 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
10
94
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…By either definition, it is clear that textures composed ofelements too small to appreciably deform the skin must be perceived in some nonspatial way, perhaps involving vibratory activation of rapidly adapting channels (Heller, 1989;Johnson & Hsiao, 1992;LaMotte & Srinivasan, 1991). Thus, studies ofcoarse-texture perception implicate spatial processing, whereas data on perception of fine textures are at least compatible with the use of vibrational cues.…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…By either definition, it is clear that textures composed ofelements too small to appreciably deform the skin must be perceived in some nonspatial way, perhaps involving vibratory activation of rapidly adapting channels (Heller, 1989;Johnson & Hsiao, 1992;LaMotte & Srinivasan, 1991). Thus, studies ofcoarse-texture perception implicate spatial processing, whereas data on perception of fine textures are at least compatible with the use of vibrational cues.…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…For example, another early study (Meenes & Zigler, 1923) reported that subjects who described moving textured stimuli as rough or smooth did not find these terms appropriate to describe the same surfaces when lateral movement was disallowed, preferring to describe their sensations in terms of even or uneven pressure; but there is no indication in their study that this was more true of fine textures than of coarse textures. More recent studies of roughness perception generally confine themselves to relatively coarse textures (Connor et aI., 1990;Connor & Johnson, 1992) or do not measure performance under conditions of static touch (Heller, 1989;Lamb, 1983;Lederman, 1974;Lederman & Taylor, 1972;Stevens & Harris, 1962;Taylor & Lederman, 1975). Thus, Phillips, Johnson, and Browne's (1983, p. 243) prediction that "texture discrimination ... that is believed to depend on information relayed by the [vibration-sensitive] rapidly adapting afferents should be profoundly affected by skin movement" has rarely been subjected to quantitative test.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, tactually perceived two-dimensional shapes may be visualized (Heller, 1989;Lederman, Thorne, & Jones, 1986). Sensory information may also be condensed so that less must be remembered, such as converting the absolute position of an object in space to relative body coordinates (e.g., updown, left-right;see Millar, 1978a).…”
Section: Recodingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They further proposed that this visual mediation is needed for the interpretation of pictures, since haptics excels in the perception of solids. Solid forms possess the substancerelated attributes that haptics does very well with, namely texture (see Heller 1989b), hardness, thermal properties, and so forth. An important line of data that supports this position derives from the finding that blindfolded sighted subjects may have difficulty naming unfamiliar tangible pictures (Lederman et al 1990), yet are able to name the identical solid objects (Klatzky et al 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%