2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.11.017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Testosterone enhances risk tolerance without altering motor impulsivity in male rats

Abstract: Summary Anabolic-androgenic steroids (AAS) increase impulsive and uncontrolled aggressive (‘roid rage) in humans and enhance agonistic behavior in animals. However, the underlying mechanisms for AAS-induced aggression remain unclear. Potential contributing elements include an increase risk-taking and/or motor impulsivity due to AAS. This study addressed the effects of chronic high-dose testosterone on risk tolerance using a risky decision-making task (RDT) and motor impulsivity with a go/no-go task in operant … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
79
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 88 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
(85 reference statements)
2
79
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The fact that there was no significant correlation between body weight and mean percent choice of the large, risky reward (controlling for sex, r = 0.18, p = 0.35) argues against this possibility [see also (Mitchell et al, 2014)]. To address this issue more directly, however, shock intensities were adjusted as a function of body weight (1.0 mA/kg) every other day in the second cohort of rats (Cooper, Goings, Kim, & Wood, 2014). Rats were tested in the RDT using these procedures until stable behavior emerged (18 sessions; Fig 3).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fact that there was no significant correlation between body weight and mean percent choice of the large, risky reward (controlling for sex, r = 0.18, p = 0.35) argues against this possibility [see also (Mitchell et al, 2014)]. To address this issue more directly, however, shock intensities were adjusted as a function of body weight (1.0 mA/kg) every other day in the second cohort of rats (Cooper, Goings, Kim, & Wood, 2014). Rats were tested in the RDT using these procedures until stable behavior emerged (18 sessions; Fig 3).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3B, rats tested in this task prefer the large reward when the probability of punishment is low, but shift their preference away from the large reward (and toward the small, safe reward) as the probability of punishment increases. The degree of preference for the large, "risky" reward is strongly dependent on shock intensity, with higher intensities resulting in decreased choice of the large reward (Simon et al, 2009;Cooper et al, 2014;Shimp et al, 2015). Somewhat surprisingly, the magnitude of the large reward appears to play less of a role in directing choice behavior in this task.…”
Section: A Rat Model Of Decision-making Under Risk Of Punishmentmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…200 g body weight at the start of the study; Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA, U.S.A.) were pair-housed with a same-sex conspecific under a reversed 14:10 h light:dark cycle. To facilitate operant responding, male rats were maintained on a slow rate of growth (3–4 g/day) during training, as in Cooper, Goings, Kim, and Wood (2014). To eliminate cyclic fluctuations in ovarian steroid hormones and maintain chronic physiologic levels of oestrogen, female rats were ovariectomized via bilateral dorsal flank incision, and received a subcutaneous 4 mm Silastic oestradiol implant (inner diameter: 1.98 mm, outer diameter: 3.18 mm; Dow Corning, Midland, MI, U.S.A.; Bridges, 1984).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%