1996
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1996.82.3c.1223
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Test Anxiety and Working Memory System

Abstract: The purpose was to examine the effect of test anxiety on deficits in the articulatory loop in the working memory system. We used a verbal memory task to occupy the articulatory loop and a spatial memory task to occupy the visuospatial scratch pad. 17 highly test-anxious and 19 low-anxiety subjects performed both tasks under an anxiety-inducing situation during which they were observed by a monitor camera. Percentage of correct responses and reaction times were measured as indices of task performance. Highly an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
66
2
2

Year Published

2001
2001
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 109 publications
(80 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
10
66
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…On the other hand, self-reported state performance-approach has also been found to predict higher capacity scores, but only when controlling for negative affect 5 (Linnenbrink, Ryan, & Pintrich, 1999). This is consistent with negative impact of anxiety on working memory performance (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001;Ikeda, Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996;MacLeod & Donnellan, 1993), as well as suggestions that performanceapproach goals may elicit negative cognitions (e.g., anxiety and worry) that undermine effective use of cognitive resources (Linnenbrink et al, 1999; see also Pintrich & Schrauben, 1992). Interestingly, analysis of working memory processing errors on RSPAN, rather than capacity scores, indicate fewer errors for participants in a no-goal control condition relative to achievement goal states (Parkes et al, 1998).…”
supporting
confidence: 49%
“…On the other hand, self-reported state performance-approach has also been found to predict higher capacity scores, but only when controlling for negative affect 5 (Linnenbrink, Ryan, & Pintrich, 1999). This is consistent with negative impact of anxiety on working memory performance (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001;Ikeda, Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996;MacLeod & Donnellan, 1993), as well as suggestions that performanceapproach goals may elicit negative cognitions (e.g., anxiety and worry) that undermine effective use of cognitive resources (Linnenbrink et al, 1999; see also Pintrich & Schrauben, 1992). Interestingly, analysis of working memory processing errors on RSPAN, rather than capacity scores, indicate fewer errors for participants in a no-goal control condition relative to achievement goal states (Parkes et al, 1998).…”
supporting
confidence: 49%
“…In line with these predictions, state anxiety was associated with the decreased modulation of picture processing by working memory load (i.e., as indexed by the LPP); however, anxiety was not associated with differential behavioral performance (for similar results, see . Recent work has used ERPs to index compensatory effort in anxiety (Ansari & Derakshan, 2011), and more studies of this kind may be useful in understanding why anxious individuals may, at times, perform on par with nonanxious individuals (Ikeda, Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996;NavehBenjamin, McKeachie, Lin, & Holinger, 1981;A. Richards, French, Keogh, & Carter, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research in the test anxiety literature indicating that the representation and rehearsal of unwanted thoughts and worries impacts the phonological resources of working memory supports this notion (Darke, 1988;Ikeda, Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996;Markham & Darke, 1991;Rapee, 1993). These thoughts and worries may also have some impact on central executive resources.…”
Section: Returning To Stereotype Threatmentioning
confidence: 95%