2019
DOI: 10.1167/19.12.13
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal dynamics of binocular integration in primary visual cortex

Abstract: Whenever we open our eyes, our brain quickly integrates the two eyes' perspectives into a combined view. This process of binocular integration happens so rapidly that even incompatible stimuli are briefly fused before one eye's view is suppressed in favor of the other (binocular rivalry). The neuronal basis for this brief period of fusion during incompatible binocular stimulation is unclear. Neuroanatomically, the eyes provide two largely separate streams of information that are integrated into a binocular res… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
32
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

6
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 95 publications
(130 reference statements)
2
32
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The size of the items in the array scaled with eccentricity at 0.3 dva per dva eccentricity so that they were smaller than the estimated overall receptive field size (Freeman and Simoncelli 2011). To determine the orientation and eccentricity of the array, each day online multiunit activity was measured during a receptive field mapping task (Cox et al 2013(Cox et al , 2019Dougherty et al 2019;Westerberg et al 2019), where the monkey fixated while a series of stimuli were presented across the visual field. The array was then oriented so that its eccentricity coincided with the location of the receptive field (3-10 dva eccentricity) and a single array item was placed at the center of the receptive field.…”
Section: Experimental Design and Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The size of the items in the array scaled with eccentricity at 0.3 dva per dva eccentricity so that they were smaller than the estimated overall receptive field size (Freeman and Simoncelli 2011). To determine the orientation and eccentricity of the array, each day online multiunit activity was measured during a receptive field mapping task (Cox et al 2013(Cox et al , 2019Dougherty et al 2019;Westerberg et al 2019), where the monkey fixated while a series of stimuli were presented across the visual field. The array was then oriented so that its eccentricity coincided with the location of the receptive field (3-10 dva eccentricity) and a single array item was placed at the center of the receptive field.…”
Section: Experimental Design and Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Orthogonal array penetrations were confirmed online through a reverse-correlation RF mapping procedure (Nandy et al, 2017;Westerberg et al, 2019;Cox et al, 2019aCox et al, , 2019bDougherty et al, 2019) (Figure S6A). RFs were found to represent portions of visual space consistent with previous reports of V4 (Gattass et al, 1988) (Figure S6D).…”
Section: Laminar Alignmentmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Supplementary Figure 2 demonstrates the reliability of these functional markers following alignment of all sessions. Both extracranial to intracranial and gray matter to white matter boundaries were determined by finding the pair of recording electrodes where no multiunit response to visual stimuli was observed on one channel and a significant response was observed on the other (Cox et al, 2019b;Westerberg et al, 2019). Recording channels positioned between these pairs all showed significant responses.…”
Section: Laminar Alignmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, we found no instances of a lack of response on a channel determined to be within the gray matter. The L2/3-L4 boundary was set to 0.5 mm above the L4-L5 boundary as we do not have a reliable functional marker and that distance is consistent with histological studies of V1 laminar structure (see Cox et al, 2019b;Westerberg et al, 2019 for details).…”
Section: Laminar Alignmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation