2012
DOI: 10.1037/a0027885
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Templates for rejection: Configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features.

Abstract: Theories of attention are compatible with the idea that we can bias attention to avoid selecting objects that have known nontarget features. Although this may underlie several existing phenomena, the explicit guidance of attention away from known nontargets has yet to be demonstrated. Here we show that observers can use feature cues (i.e., color) to bias attention away from nontarget items during visual search. These negative cues were used to quickly instantiate a template for rejection that reliably facilita… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

28
326
3
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 188 publications
(358 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
28
326
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One piece of evidence for reactive suppression of distractors is provided by the distractor positivity (Pd), an event-related potential that occurs contralateral to a suppressed location (Hickey, Di Lollo, & McDonald, 2009). There is also evidence for proactive control of distractor suppression, as observers try to adopt an attentional set against distractor locations or distractor features (e.g., Arita, Carlisle, & Woodman, 2012;Awh, Matsukura, & Serences, 2003;Poole & Kane, 2009;Reeder, Olivers, & Pollmann, 2017). Our results extend these findings by demonstrating that observers can use available information not only to avoid attending unwanted stimuli, but also to facilitate the shift of attention toward stimuli that match a current task set.…”
Section: The Role Of Cognitive Control In Visual Searchsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…One piece of evidence for reactive suppression of distractors is provided by the distractor positivity (Pd), an event-related potential that occurs contralateral to a suppressed location (Hickey, Di Lollo, & McDonald, 2009). There is also evidence for proactive control of distractor suppression, as observers try to adopt an attentional set against distractor locations or distractor features (e.g., Arita, Carlisle, & Woodman, 2012;Awh, Matsukura, & Serences, 2003;Poole & Kane, 2009;Reeder, Olivers, & Pollmann, 2017). Our results extend these findings by demonstrating that observers can use available information not only to avoid attending unwanted stimuli, but also to facilitate the shift of attention toward stimuli that match a current task set.…”
Section: The Role Of Cognitive Control In Visual Searchsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…In Experiment 3a, we found that discrimination training with the target prior to search task eliminated the benefit of the context cue on search. Although distractor suppression may exist in other situations (Arita et al 2012;Moher et al, 2014), the preponderance of evidence in this study suggests that the context cues were not used for distractor suppression.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Recent studies have reported distractor suppression based on salient features (Arita, Carlisle & Woodman, 2012;Moher, Lakshmanan, Egeth & Ewen, 2014). To test whether our observers were using the context cues to inhibit the processing of the distractors, we repeated the experiment with the same three distractors and ten randomly varying targets.…”
Section: Experiments 2: Distractor Suppression?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Top-down guidance is even strong enough to override attention capture by low-level salience (Chen & Zelinsky, 2006). In essence, top-down guidance works in two ways: it biases attention toward important features or regions, and it biases attention away from undesirable features (or objects that have already been inspected; Al-Aidroos et al, 2012; Arita, Carlisle, & Woodman 2012). It is unsurprising that successful models of visual search, such as Guided Search (Wolfe et al, 1989; Wolfe, 1994; Wolfe & Gancarz, 1996, Wolfe, 2007; Palmer et al, 2011), incorporate top-down guidance as a key mechanism controlling attention.…”
Section: Guidance Of Attention By Bottom-up and Top-down Informationmentioning
confidence: 99%