2005
DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20191
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Telling truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event‐related fMRI

Abstract: R. (2005). Telling the truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event-related fMRI. Retrieved from http://repository.upenn.edu/neuroethics_pubs/6Telling the truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event-related f MRI AbstractDeception is a clinically important behavior with poorly understood neurobiological correlates. Published functional MRI (fMRI) data on the brain activity during deception indicates that, on a multisubject group level, lie is distinguished from truth by increased prefrontal … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

15
211
3
3

Year Published

2006
2006
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 269 publications
(232 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
15
211
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition to the work of Petrides [1994] and others, this result ties in well with the view that the mid-VLPFC is activated under a wide variety of conditions that require control processes (Fig. 3 and Table I; [Braver et al, 2003;Bunge et al, 2001;Cadoret et al, 2001;Cools et al, 2002;Dove et al, 2000Dove et al, , 2006Duncan and Owen, 2000;Houde et al, 2000;Jenkins et al, 1994;Kostopoulos and Petrides, 2003;Langleben et al, 2005;Turk et al, 2004]). Note that the coordinates reported in these studies are anatomically very close to the centre of our regions of interest in the mid-VLPFC (Fig.…”
Section: Mid-vlpfc Activitysupporting
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition to the work of Petrides [1994] and others, this result ties in well with the view that the mid-VLPFC is activated under a wide variety of conditions that require control processes (Fig. 3 and Table I; [Braver et al, 2003;Bunge et al, 2001;Cadoret et al, 2001;Cools et al, 2002;Dove et al, 2000Dove et al, , 2006Duncan and Owen, 2000;Houde et al, 2000;Jenkins et al, 1994;Kostopoulos and Petrides, 2003;Langleben et al, 2005;Turk et al, 2004]). Note that the coordinates reported in these studies are anatomically very close to the centre of our regions of interest in the mid-VLPFC (Fig.…”
Section: Mid-vlpfc Activitysupporting
confidence: 83%
“…This region -spreading from the outer surface along the frontal operculum to become continuous with reported activations in the anterior insula, close to the coordinates À41, 20, 0 and 37, 20, 3, [Duncan and Owen, 2000] -has shown activity in a broad range of tasks requiring control processes, from attention [Duncan and Owen, 2000], memory [Owen, 2000] and task switching [Dove et al, 2000] to less frequently studied tasks such as ''choosing a dinner date'' [Turk et al, 2004] and ''lying'' [Langleben et al, 2005], i.e. a range of tasks with different stimuli and contexts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…In addition to regional cortical activity differing as a function of deceptive response type, our event-related fMRI investigation is one of two investigations to observe greater activation in the normal response condition relative to deception [21]. Similar to that investigation, our findings of greater dorsomedial parietal and occipital activation during normal recognition may reflect alternate visual object recognition and attentional resources that are otherwise not called into action or are suppressed during purposefully deceptive response modes.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 69%
“…Spence et al discovered activation in ventrolateral, dorsomedial, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (VLPFC, DMPFC, and DLPFC, respectively), as well as the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), during deceptive relative to normal responding for both auditory and visual stimuli. These regions have been commonly associated with deceptive responding in fMRI studies that followed and appear to reflect the generation and inhibition of responses, increased working memory load, meta-cognition of task performance, and monitoring of social cues necessary to deceive others [17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26]. With the exception of two studies to be discussed below [17,23], the bulk of neuroimaging and ERP investigations into deceptive behavior have focused upon the neuroanatomical correlates involved in the suppression of prior knowledge by employing methodological variations of the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) [27].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of studies have used functional MRI (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) to identify the neural substrate supporting deceptive behavior. These have used a number of deceptive tasks and scenarios, including guilty knowledge tasks (GKT: Langleben et al, 2002Langleben et al, , 2005Phan et al, 2005), mock crime scenarios (Kozel, Padgett, & George, 2004;Kozel et al, 2005;Mohamed et al, 2006), feigned memory impairment (Lee et al, 2002(Lee et al, , 2005, and autobiographical or experienced events (Abe et al, 2006;Abe, Suzuki, Mori, Itoh, & Fujii, 2007;Ganis, Kosslyn, Stose, Thompson, & Yurgelun-Todd, 2003;Nuň ez, Casey, Egner, Harre, & Hirsch, 2005;Spence et al, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%