2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0013299
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tell-tale eyes: Children's attribution of gaze aversion as a lying cue.

Abstract: This study examined whether the well-documented adult tendency to perceive gaze aversion as a lying cue is also evident in children. In Experiment 1, 6-year-olds, 9-year-olds, and adults were shown video vignettes of speakers who either maintained or avoided eye contact while answering an interviewer's questions. Participants evaluated whether the speaker was telling the truth or lying on each trial. The results revealed that at both ages, children were more likely to attribute lying to speakers in the gaze av… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 70 publications
(109 reference statements)
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the current experiment, 6-year-olds perceived eye contact over a wider horizontal range of positions than did older children and adults and, thus, may be less sensitive to the social signals associated with averted gaze. This, along with less knowledge of the display rules associated with eye gaze (see McCarthy & Lee, 2009), could be why 6-year-olds are less likely than 9-year-olds and adults to attribute deception to a person displaying averted gaze (Einav & Hood, 2008). Children's sensitivity to the other social signals associated with averted gaze (e.g., attention toward an event in the environment, avoidance) has not been reported.…”
Section: Developmentmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the current experiment, 6-year-olds perceived eye contact over a wider horizontal range of positions than did older children and adults and, thus, may be less sensitive to the social signals associated with averted gaze. This, along with less knowledge of the display rules associated with eye gaze (see McCarthy & Lee, 2009), could be why 6-year-olds are less likely than 9-year-olds and adults to attribute deception to a person displaying averted gaze (Einav & Hood, 2008). Children's sensitivity to the other social signals associated with averted gaze (e.g., attention toward an event in the environment, avoidance) has not been reported.…”
Section: Developmentmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Both 9-year-olds and adults attribute deception to a person who fails to make eye contact (Einav & Hood, 2008;see also McCarthy & Lee, 2009). Like adults, 8-to 15-yearolds are faster at detecting faces presented with direct gaze than at detecting faces presented with averted gaze , and 6-to 11-year-olds are better at remembering facial identity when gaze is direct (Smith et al, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Although 4-year-olds could keep in mind who had made greater numerical errors in the past when evaluating new claims, it was not until age 6 or 7 that children succeeded at applying an understanding of less measurable errors (e.g., whether it is worse to call a tiger a mouse or a lion). Furthermore, only 7-year-olds used claims made by the informant who had previously made smaller errors to influence their own answers (Einav & Robinson, 2010). In other words, by age 4, children can reflect on the degree of someone’s errors, but there are still developmental improvements in sensitivity to the degree of errors in different domains and in recognizing that some errors can be informative.…”
Section: What Is Developing and What Is Stable?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, if eye-closure or gaze aversion benefit cognition by individuals who suffer acutely from social anxiety or shyness, it may be advantageous to encourage these individuals to adopt these techniques in relevant settings (e.g., in the classroom). However, it is also important to note that negative social judgments frequently are made of individuals who avert their gaze or turn away from an interlocutor (e.g., Larsen and Shackelford, 1996), especially as people who avert their gaze are often perceived as deceptive by others (for discussion see, e.g., Vrij and Semin, 1996; Mann et al, 2002; Einav and Hood, 2008). Finally, as noted already, the present studies used only adult participants.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%