2011
DOI: 10.1075/eww.32.2.04sch
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Teenagers’ acquisition of variation

Abstract: Variation conferences have provided helpful formative feedback. We alone are responsible for any failings in this paper. The high schools we worked in remain anonymous, but we owe a debt of great magnitude to the staff and students in Edinburgh and London who worked with us on this project. We admire and respect the individuals in these schools for the work they are doing. In particular, our thanks go to the migrant teenagers, for some of whom the interview and reading tasks were a major effort. Wielkie dzieki… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
30
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
3
30
1
Order By: Relevance
“…It seems that social class interacts with regional area and the baseline frequency of [ɪn] in a particular area (e.g., Labov, 2001:90). A number of studies exemplify this point for locations in Britain such as London (Schleef et al, 2011), Sandwell, West Midlands (Mathisen, 1999, Wilmslow, Cheshire (Watts, 2005), Manchester (Schleef, Flynn, & Ramsammy, 2015), Cardiff (Mees, 1977), Norwich (Trudgill, 1974), York (Tagliamonte, 2004), and Edinburgh (Romaine, 1984;Schleef et al, 2011). Data from some of these studies suggest that the lower middle classes in northern cities stand out as relatively low users of [ɪŋ], with numbers often as low as for working class speakers in southern cities.…”
Section: Figure 1: Modern [ɪŋ] ~ [ɪN] Alternation As Probabilities Fomentioning
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It seems that social class interacts with regional area and the baseline frequency of [ɪn] in a particular area (e.g., Labov, 2001:90). A number of studies exemplify this point for locations in Britain such as London (Schleef et al, 2011), Sandwell, West Midlands (Mathisen, 1999, Wilmslow, Cheshire (Watts, 2005), Manchester (Schleef, Flynn, & Ramsammy, 2015), Cardiff (Mees, 1977), Norwich (Trudgill, 1974), York (Tagliamonte, 2004), and Edinburgh (Romaine, 1984;Schleef et al, 2011). Data from some of these studies suggest that the lower middle classes in northern cities stand out as relatively low users of [ɪŋ], with numbers often as low as for working class speakers in southern cities.…”
Section: Figure 1: Modern [ɪŋ] ~ [ɪN] Alternation As Probabilities Fomentioning
confidence: 91%
“…The presence of [ɪn] decreases in more formal speech styles (Hazen, 2006;Labov, 2001;Reid, 1978;Romaine, 1984;Schleef et al, 2011;Trudgill, 1974). In varieties where [ɪŋɡ] occurs as a third variant of (ing), as is the case for Manchester English, [ɪŋɡ] too is more plentiful in formal styles (Mathisen, 1999;Watts, 2005).…”
Section: Figure 1: Modern [ɪŋ] ~ [ɪN] Alternation As Probabilities Fomentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Schleef, Meyerhoff & Clark (2011) examined Polish teenagers" acquisition of variation in Edinburgh and London by comparing groups of students from the same schools (see also Clark & Schleef 2010;Meyerhoff & Schleef 2012). Although there were differences between the two groups, they found that the Polish teenagers were acquiring native speakers" productions of (ing), especially those who had a mixture of Polish and British friends.…”
Section: The Role Of the Peer Groupmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, as reviewed in the introduction section, there is extensive evidence from sociolinguistic research which shows that the speech patterns of adolescents are crucially affected by the peer group to which they belong (Eckert, 1989(Eckert, , 2000Morris, 2013;Nance, 2013Nance, , 2014Nance, , 2015Schleef et al, 2011), with peer group identity being marked by specific speech patterns.…”
Section: Linguistic Experience Peer Group Identity and Welshnessmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, Polish immigrant adolescents in the UK showed lower rates of the non-standard [ɪn] variant of the (ing) variable than their locally-born peers in London and Edinburgh (Schleef et al 2011), and higher rates of standard [ɪŋ]. 5 Again, the non-native learners failed to acquire the appropriate linguistic and sociolinguistic constraints on the variation from naturalistic input, but overall produced less non-standard-like speech than the native speakers.…”
Section: Standardisation In Naturalistic Language Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%