1971
DOI: 10.1080/00221309.1971.9711319
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Task Difficulty and Noncontingent Reinforcement Schedules as Factors in Self-Reinforcement

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

1971
1971
1979
1979

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A comparison of current findings with those reported by Dorsey, Kanfer, and Duerfeldt (1971) points to a major difference in SR behavior if a contingent as opposed to a noncontingent feedback procedure is employed during training. While both procedures lecl to increases in CSRs with increases in external feedback, increases in ISRs with increases in external feedback occurred only when the noncontingent feedback technique was used.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 58%
“…A comparison of current findings with those reported by Dorsey, Kanfer, and Duerfeldt (1971) points to a major difference in SR behavior if a contingent as opposed to a noncontingent feedback procedure is employed during training. While both procedures lecl to increases in CSRs with increases in external feedback, increases in ISRs with increases in external feedback occurred only when the noncontingent feedback technique was used.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 58%
“…Internals received 54% selfreinforcement and 57% experimenter reinforcement, and externals received 70% self-reinforcement and 63% experimenter reinforcement. Dorsey, Kanfer, and Duerfeldt (1971) and Duerfeldt (1967a, 1967b) have shown that self-reinforcement rate is strongly influenced by prior experi-ence with experimenter reinforcement. However, the effects are influenced by the perceived relevance of experimenter reinforcement.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kanfer's research has found that one's rate and accuracy of selfreward tends to be similar to the rate and accuracy of external re ward one has previously received (Bartol & Duerfeldt, 1970;Kanfer, Bradley & Marston, 1962;Kanfer & Duerfeldt, 1967b;Dorsey, Kanfer & Duerfeldt, 1971;Simkins & Kingery, 1970). Self-reward rates tend to be either unrelated or negatively related to self-punishment rates in the same person (Kanfer & Duerfeldt, 1967b;Kanfer, Duerfeldt & LePage, I969).…”
Section: Theoretical Interprétations Of Self-controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, no consistent definition of self-reward or selfpunishment has been used across the various laboratory analogue stud ies, Hence, in the following review, self-reward and self-punishment are defined as the application of an appetitive or aversive stimulus following a response. The stimulus may be self-selected, selfadministered, or self-managed (e.g., Bass, 1972;Skinner, 1953;Thoresen & Mahoney, 197^;Weiner & Dubanoski, 1975)• Self-applied consequences in laboratory analogue studies have included tangible (e.g., candy), conditioned (e.g., a green light), and covert (e.g., self-commendation and self-criticism) stimuli (Weiner & Dubanoski, 1975;Dorsey, Kanfer & Duerfeldt, 1971;Haynes & Kanfer, 1971;respectively). In the present review, the terms reinforcer and reward are used synonymously.…”
Section: Introduction To Behavioral Self-controlmentioning
confidence: 99%