2000
DOI: 10.2307/40285899
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tapping in Time with Mechanically and Expressively Performed Music

Abstract: We investigate how the presence of performance microstructure (small variations in timing, intensity, and articulation) influences listeners' perception of musical excerpts, by measuring the way in which listeners synchronize with the excerpts. Musicians and nonmusicians tapped on a drum in synchrony with six musical excerpts, each presented in three versions: mechanical (synthesized from the score, without microstructure), accented (mechanical, with intensity accents), and expressive (performed by a concert p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

10
169
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 179 publications
(179 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
10
169
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, in a study that combined structural equation modeling and covariance analysis, Magill and Pressing (1997) were able to demonstrate that a cognitive clock model based on nonisochronous pulses provided a good fit for most of the polyrhythmic patterns performed by an expert master drummer from West Africa. More generally, musicians have been found to exhibit enhanced skills in rhythmic tasks, such as lower thresholds for detecting timing deviations (Rammsayer & Altenmüller, 2006;Jones et al, 1995), access to a larger number of pulse levels (Drake, Jones, & Baruch, 2000;Drake, Penel, & Bigand, 2000), and greater facility in finding and subdividing an implied beat (Jones & Yee, 1997;Palmer & Krumhansl, 1990). Furthermore, although they are more sensitive to timing deviations, musicians are also more resilient to timing irregularities in pulse attribution tasks (Madison & Merker, 2002).…”
Section: Mensural Determinacymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For example, in a study that combined structural equation modeling and covariance analysis, Magill and Pressing (1997) were able to demonstrate that a cognitive clock model based on nonisochronous pulses provided a good fit for most of the polyrhythmic patterns performed by an expert master drummer from West Africa. More generally, musicians have been found to exhibit enhanced skills in rhythmic tasks, such as lower thresholds for detecting timing deviations (Rammsayer & Altenmüller, 2006;Jones et al, 1995), access to a larger number of pulse levels (Drake, Jones, & Baruch, 2000;Drake, Penel, & Bigand, 2000), and greater facility in finding and subdividing an implied beat (Jones & Yee, 1997;Palmer & Krumhansl, 1990). Furthermore, although they are more sensitive to timing deviations, musicians are also more resilient to timing irregularities in pulse attribution tasks (Madison & Merker, 2002).…”
Section: Mensural Determinacymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, in most psychological studies, meter is tied to tactus, that is, a comfortable pulse level whose rate falls within boundaries that are closely related to bodily rhythms such as heart rate and walking speed (≈ 67-200 beats per minute, which corresponds to IOIs of 300-900 msec; e.g., see Todd, Cousins, & Lee, 2007). Given the central role tapping studies have played in shaping our understanding of pulse (and meter) perception, and that, with a few notable exceptions (e.g., Drake, Penel, & Bigand, 2000;Martens, 2011;Snyder & Krumhansl, 2001;Toiviainen & Snyder, 2003;Vuust et al, 2011), most of the stimuli used in these studies are not from actual music, we might ask how widely applicable these findings are. [3] The present study explores the phenomenon of mensural determinacy in complex rhythmic sequences.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Perception of rhythm is associated with the identification of a pulse, which occurs at evenly spaced moments in time (Cooper and Meyer, 1960;Lerdahl and Jackendoff, 1983). While it is common when individuals hear music for them to move to the beat (Drake, et al, 2000), it implies the potential importance of motor networks.…”
Section: Movement Research In Music Educationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given that the reviewers seem to agree that the loss of some degree of ecological validity may be unavoidable in using experimental methods, and that the manipulations employed in the current study are well represented in previous beat finding experiments using actual music as source materials (e.g., Drake, Penel, & Bigand, 2000;Snyder & Krumhansl, 2001;Toiviainen & Snyder, 2003), my response to their commentary will focus mainly on the first two issues raised. The goal of this response is to clarify the methodology used in relation to the research questions and source materials as well as to shed some light on the implications of the findings for future research.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%