1999
DOI: 10.1007/bf03395308
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tactual Equivalence Class Formation and Tactual-to-Visual Cross-Modal Transfer

Abstract: Five adults, two who were deaf-blind and three who were hearing-sighted but blindfolded for the experiment, learned the conditional discriminations AB and BC that consisted of complex tactual stimuli. Class-consistent responding to symmetry, transitivity, and equivalence probes demonstrated the emergence of tactual equivalence classes by four of the subjects. These data confirm that visual or auditory stimuli are not needed for the induction of equivalence classes . They also suggest that equivalence class pro… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
7
0
4

Year Published

2001
2001
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
7
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…In most of these experiments, the classes have been constructed of meaningless stimuli to minimize the effects of stimulus content on class formation. Most of the classes studied were composed of stimuli from visual, auditory and/or olfactory sensory modalities (e.g., Annette & Leslie, 1995;Belanich & Fields, 1999;Fienup & Dixon, 2006;Green, 1990;Hayes, Tilley, & Hayes, 19898, Sidman, 2009). When visual stimuli were the class members, they were usually abstract, and consisted of nonsense syllables such as 3-letter consonantvowel-consonant strings with no meaning in the lexicon (e.g., Moss-Lourenco & Fields, 2011;Plaud, 1995), symbols from an alphabet with which the participants were not familiar, such as Greek or Cyrillic letters (Arntzen, 2004;Fields, Arntzen, Nartey, & Eilifsen, 2012;Holth & Arntzen, 1998), or hard-to-name shapes (Bentall, Dickins, & Fox, 1993).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In most of these experiments, the classes have been constructed of meaningless stimuli to minimize the effects of stimulus content on class formation. Most of the classes studied were composed of stimuli from visual, auditory and/or olfactory sensory modalities (e.g., Annette & Leslie, 1995;Belanich & Fields, 1999;Fienup & Dixon, 2006;Green, 1990;Hayes, Tilley, & Hayes, 19898, Sidman, 2009). When visual stimuli were the class members, they were usually abstract, and consisted of nonsense syllables such as 3-letter consonantvowel-consonant strings with no meaning in the lexicon (e.g., Moss-Lourenco & Fields, 2011;Plaud, 1995), symbols from an alphabet with which the participants were not familiar, such as Greek or Cyrillic letters (Arntzen, 2004;Fields, Arntzen, Nartey, & Eilifsen, 2012;Holth & Arntzen, 1998), or hard-to-name shapes (Bentall, Dickins, & Fox, 1993).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The sensory modality of the stimuli used as class members-for instance, visual (e.g., Fields, Arntzen, Nartey, & Eilifsen, 2012), tactile (e.g., Belanich & Fields, 1999), and gustatory (e.g., Hayes, Tilley, & Hayes, 1988)-has also been shown to influence the formation of equivalence classes. When visual stimuli were used as the class members, they consisted of abstract shapes (e.g., Sidman & Tailby, 1982) or meaningful pictures (e.g., Arntzen, 2004;Fields et al, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Stimulus equivalence has been demonstrated w ith both verbally competent people (Dugdale & Lowe, 2000), including adults and ty pically developing ch ildren (e.g., A rntzen & Vaidya, 2008;Lipkins, Hayes, & Hayes, 1993;Pilgrim, Chambers, & Galizio, 1995;Sidman & Tailby, 1982), and those with developmental disabilities or autism (Arntzen, Halstadtro, Bjerke, & Halstadtro, 2010;LeBlanc, Miguel, Cummings, Goldsmith, & Carr, 2003). It has also been demonstrated after training with different stimulus modalities such as olfactory (Annett & Leslie, 1995), haptic (Belanich & Fields, 1999), tactile (O'Leary & Bush, 1996), and gustatory (L. J. Hayes, Tilley, & Hayes, 1988).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%