2006
DOI: 10.1080/10683160500337550
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Suggestibility and children with mild learning disabilities: The use of the cognitive interview

Abstract: The aims of this paper were firstly to identify any differences in the level of suggestibility between 20 7 Á9-year-old children with mild learning disabilities and 20 children with average academic ability using the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale 2 (GSS2) and, secondly, to note the impact of the cognitive interview on the response patterns of children with mild learning disabilities.On the GSS2, average academic ability children recalled significantly more correct details than children with mild learning dis… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Further, this effect has been found across various populations (e.g. children: Akehurst, Milne, & Kö hnken, 2003;Holliday, 2003;Holliday & Albon, 2004;Memon, Wark, Bull, & Kö hnken, 1997; children with mild learning difficulties: Robinson & McGuire, 2006;young adults: Fisher et al, 1987;Fisher, Geiselman, & Amador, 1989; and the elderly: Wright & Holliday, 2007). Some studies have reported a slight increase in the amount of incorrect information recalled using the CI nevertheless this has not affected the overall CI accuracy rates (proportion of correct details relative to the total amount of details reported) compared to other forensic interview procedures (see Köhnken, Milne, Memon, & Bull, 1999 for a metaanalysis).…”
Section: The Cognitive Interviewmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Further, this effect has been found across various populations (e.g. children: Akehurst, Milne, & Kö hnken, 2003;Holliday, 2003;Holliday & Albon, 2004;Memon, Wark, Bull, & Kö hnken, 1997; children with mild learning difficulties: Robinson & McGuire, 2006;young adults: Fisher et al, 1987;Fisher, Geiselman, & Amador, 1989; and the elderly: Wright & Holliday, 2007). Some studies have reported a slight increase in the amount of incorrect information recalled using the CI nevertheless this has not affected the overall CI accuracy rates (proportion of correct details relative to the total amount of details reported) compared to other forensic interview procedures (see Köhnken, Milne, Memon, & Bull, 1999 for a metaanalysis).…”
Section: The Cognitive Interviewmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Next, they answered standardized ‘who, what, when, where, how’ open-ended questions about their experiences performing the everyday life situation; these questions primed their memory and assisted with recall and reflection. 14,15 An item set for that situation was then administered on a tablet using touch-activated PowerPoint (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA). Each participant chose a second everyday life situation and repeated this process.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The originality of the CI consists in its composition that combines social and cognitive (i.e., mnemonic) instructions (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992). Its effectiveness has been widely demonstrated, that is in many countries and with various populations (e.g., elderly persons: Wright & Holliday, 2007; adults: Dando, Wilcock, Milne, & Henry, 2009;Fisher, Geiselman, Raymond, Jurkewich, & Warhaftig, 1987;children: Holliday & Albon, 2004;Milne & Bull, 2002;; and people with learning disabilities: Milne & Bull, 1996;Robinson & McGuire, 2006). In addition to its benefit on witnesses' recollection, Fisher and Geiselman (2010) suggested that the CI could have benefits on their well-being.…”
Section: R é S U M émentioning
confidence: 98%