2019
DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2019.8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Style of pictorial representation is shaped by intergroup contact

Abstract: Abstract

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
(74 reference statements)
0
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Kegl et al, 1999;Meir et al, 2010;Brentari & Goldin-Meadow, 2017; inter alia), and studied experimentally in the laboratory (e.g. Fay et al, 2013;Schouwstra & de Swart, 2014;Granito et al, 2019;Motamedi et al, 2019;Raviv et al, 2019;inter alia). What is commonly observed in these literatures is how behaviour that is sufficiently similar to previously successfully informative behaviour tends to be interpreted by audiences as a further token of the same type as previously used, even after just one interaction; and also how further repetition causes the focal behaviours to become increasingly conventional.…”
Section: Languagesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kegl et al, 1999;Meir et al, 2010;Brentari & Goldin-Meadow, 2017; inter alia), and studied experimentally in the laboratory (e.g. Fay et al, 2013;Schouwstra & de Swart, 2014;Granito et al, 2019;Motamedi et al, 2019;Raviv et al, 2019;inter alia). What is commonly observed in these literatures is how behaviour that is sufficiently similar to previously successfully informative behaviour tends to be interpreted by audiences as a further token of the same type as previously used, even after just one interaction; and also how further repetition causes the focal behaviours to become increasingly conventional.…”
Section: Languagesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although art is a medium of communication and assorting with others with similar social norms or behaviors can facilitate successful coordination and cooperation, we believe that styles in art in this context do not play a coordinating role. One possible reason for the lack of covariation of artistic styles along cultural boundaries is that artists might have grown more similar to each other over time due to increased interactions (Healey et al, 2007;Granito et al, 2019). Due to the crosssectional nature of our data, we might have not been able to see such a change over time.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Due to the crosssectional nature of our data, we might have not been able to see such a change over time. However, since the kolam tradition is considered community knowledge and artists share their designs with each other (Nagarajan, 2018), the development of styles might have been influenced by frequent intergroup contact, and shared attention and learning (Tomasello et al, 1993;Granito et al, 2019). Precisely, the notion of kolam art as community knowledge might have led to pressures to make artistic styles accessible and transparent to any potential audience.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, there has been growing interest in empirically studying the general mechanisms of musical change (Mehr et al, 2019;Ravignani et al, 2016;Youngblood et al, 2021) and, more broadly, artistic change (Fraiberger et al, 2018;Granito et al, 2019;Sobchuk, 2018). These mechanisms tend to fall into two broad categories.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%