2010
DOI: 10.1007/s10763-010-9209-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Students’ Evaluation of the Credibility of Scientific Models That Represent Natural Entities and Phenomena

Abstract: The purpose of the current study was to explore learners' evaluation of the credibility of scientific models that represent natural entities and phenomena. Participants were 845 students in grades 9-11 (aged 15-17 years) and 108 prospective science teachers in Oman, totaling 953 students. A survey called Epistemologies about the Credibility of Scientific Models was designed to explore participants' epistemological positions regarding the credibility of scientific models. This instrument was based on a credibil… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
23
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
3

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
(89 reference statements)
3
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Also, the presence of difficult, new and/ or low frequency words within the text is associated with longer gaze durations and leads to longer total inspection times (Sereno and Rayner, 2003;Smallwood et al, 2008;Foulsham et al, 2013), contributing to a slower reading pace (Foulsham et al, 2013) and worse reading comprehension (Smallwood et al, 2008). They are more abstract (Gericke and Hagberg, 2007;Al-Balushi, 2011, 2013bAl-Balushi and Coll, 2013;Taber, 2013), less frequently encountered by learners than macroscopic description of natural phenomena and they represent more difficult concepts than macroscopic words (Sanger et al, 2013;Kelly, 2014;Milenković et al, 2014;Prilliman, 2014;Ryan and Herrington, 2014;Sjöström and Talanquer, 2014;Warfa et al, 2014). They are more abstract (Gericke and Hagberg, 2007;Al-Balushi, 2011, 2013bAl-Balushi and Coll, 2013;Taber, 2013), less frequently encountered by learners than macroscopic description of natural phenomena and they represent more difficult concepts than macroscopic words (Sanger et al, 2013;Kelly, 2014;Milenković et al, 2014;Prilliman, 2014;Ryan and Herrington, 2014;Sjöström and Talanquer, 2014;Warfa et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Also, the presence of difficult, new and/ or low frequency words within the text is associated with longer gaze durations and leads to longer total inspection times (Sereno and Rayner, 2003;Smallwood et al, 2008;Foulsham et al, 2013), contributing to a slower reading pace (Foulsham et al, 2013) and worse reading comprehension (Smallwood et al, 2008). They are more abstract (Gericke and Hagberg, 2007;Al-Balushi, 2011, 2013bAl-Balushi and Coll, 2013;Taber, 2013), less frequently encountered by learners than macroscopic description of natural phenomena and they represent more difficult concepts than macroscopic words (Sanger et al, 2013;Kelly, 2014;Milenković et al, 2014;Prilliman, 2014;Ryan and Herrington, 2014;Sjöström and Talanquer, 2014;Warfa et al, 2014). They are more abstract (Gericke and Hagberg, 2007;Al-Balushi, 2011, 2013bAl-Balushi and Coll, 2013;Taber, 2013), less frequently encountered by learners than macroscopic description of natural phenomena and they represent more difficult concepts than macroscopic words (Sanger et al, 2013;Kelly, 2014;Milenković et al, 2014;Prilliman, 2014;Ryan and Herrington, 2014;Sjöström and Talanquer, 2014;Warfa et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Collectively, the research results discussed above reveal that learners' cognitive processing and conceptualization of entities and phenomena at the macroscopic and submicroscopic levels in chemistry are related to the level of abstractness (Gericke and Hagberg, 2007;Al-Balushi, 2011, 2013bAl-Balushi and Coll, 2013;Taber, 2013), cognitive load (Cranford et al, 2014;Gulacar et al, 2014;Springer, 2014), learners' estimation of size and scale at both levels (Tretter et al, 2006;Jones and Taylor, 2009;Jones et al, 2011) learners' distrust of the existence of scientific entities (Al-Balushi, 2011, 2013b and spatial ability required (Carter et al, 1987;Pribyl and Bodner, 1987;Yang et al, 2003;Wu and Shah, 2004;Wang and Barrow, 2011). Due to the differences between macroscopic and submicroscopic levels, the current study focuses on finding whether mind wandering can be added to the list of these differences (mentioned above) and eventually contributes to our interpretations of students' comprehension.…”
Section: Cognitive Processing Of Macroscopic and Submicroscopic Levelmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Theoretical scientific models represent a conceptual hindrance for school students because of their level of abstraction (Snir, Smith, & Raz, ) and hypothetical nature (Al‐Balushi, ; Mathewson, ). It has been reported that a considerable percentage of students from Grade 9 through to university level doubt the accuracy of the scientific representations and visual models that are used to illustrate some highly abstract and theoretical constructs, such as the electron cloud, photons, sodium chloride crystals, and chemical bonding.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been reported that a considerable percentage of students from Grade 9 through to university level doubt the accuracy of the scientific representations and visual models that are used to illustrate some highly abstract and theoretical constructs, such as the electron cloud, photons, sodium chloride crystals, and chemical bonding. Some go further and disbelieve in the existence of these constructs (Al‐Balushi, , ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation