2010
DOI: 10.1177/0022429410370785
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Student Musicians’ Ear-Playing Ability as a Function of Vernacular Music Experiences

Abstract: This study explored the differences in ear-playing ability between formal “classical” musicians and those with vernacular music experience ( N = 24). Participants heard melodies and performed them back, either by singing or playing on their instruments. The authors tracked the number of times through the listen-then-perform cycle that each participant needed for accurate performance. Participants retrospectively reported their thoughts and provided biographical information related to vernacular music experienc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

4
60
0
4

Year Published

2013
2013
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
(17 reference statements)
4
60
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, the intensiveness and type of participants' musical training was not accounted for in the Posedel et al (2011), Marques et al (2007), and Sadakata andSekiyama (2011) studies (cf., Woody &Lehmann, 2010) and neither was absolute pitch (AP) in any of the studies. AP ability is the extreme of musical expertise, and therefore due to the exceptionally enhanced acoustical awareness and brain connectivity may benefit language skills even more than ordinary musical expertise (cf., Loui, Li, Hohmann, & Schlaug, 2011).…”
Section: Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…Moreover, the intensiveness and type of participants' musical training was not accounted for in the Posedel et al (2011), Marques et al (2007), and Sadakata andSekiyama (2011) studies (cf., Woody &Lehmann, 2010) and neither was absolute pitch (AP) in any of the studies. AP ability is the extreme of musical expertise, and therefore due to the exceptionally enhanced acoustical awareness and brain connectivity may benefit language skills even more than ordinary musical expertise (cf., Loui, Li, Hohmann, & Schlaug, 2011).…”
Section: Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…A musician that reproduces the melodic sequence produced by a singing bird or fellow musician when she plucks strings, presses piano keys, or uses air to make a reed vibrate, is also imitating the sounds non-vocally (Clarke, 1993;Clarke & Baker-Short, 1987). Many musicians learn to play songs "by ear," which involves transforming heard sounds into the motor acts required to reproduce them (McPherson & Gabrielsson, 2002;Woody & Lehmann, 2010). Musicians and non-musicians can readily imitate the intonation patterns of sentences by moving a stylus on a tablet (d'Alessandro, Rilliard, & Le Beux, 2011).…”
Section: Imitating Sounds Non-vocallymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ear-playing has also been said to enhance aural development (Hallam, et al, 2012;Woody & Lehmann, 2010) and enjoyment through musical exploration (Harwood & Marsh, 2012;Priest, 1985). Currently there is a wealth of research in music education that explores the teaching and learning of popular music, including investigations of students responses to ear-playing tasks during the early stages of learning a classical instrument (McPherson, 1997(McPherson, , 2005; strategies employed by ear players to hear and play chord progressions when playing unfamiliar rock songs (Johansson, 2004); learner musicians ear-playing ability as a function of vernacular music experiences (Woody & Lehmann, 2010);…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%