1991
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1991.56-97
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Stimulus Generalization, Discrimination Learning, and Peak Shift in Horses

Abstract: Using horses, we investigated three aspects of the stimulus control of lever-pressing behavior: stimulus generalization, discrimination learning, and peak shift. Nine solid black circles, ranging in size from 0.5 in. to 4.5 in. (1.3 cm to 11.4 cm) served as stimuli. Each horse was shaped, using successive approximations, to press a rat lever with its lip in the presence of a positive stimulus, the 2.5-in. (6.4-cm) circle. Shaping proceeded quickly and was comparable to that of other laboratory organisms. After… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
2
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…When matching stimuli were placed farther apart or when the background stimulus was changed from the initial experimental conditions (from an uneven wooden surface with beams to a flat gray panel), horses could generalize information to these novel environments and presentations (Flannery, 1997). Dougherty and Lewis (1991),asked horses to discriminate between a black circle 2.5 inches in diameter (the positive or rewarded stimulus) and a black circle 1.5 inches in diameter (the negative stimulus). Interestingly, when horses were presented with intermediate stimuli Dougherty and Lewis (1991) found that, unlike pigeons and humans, horses showed symmetrical generalization gradients (they chose stimuli that were closest in size to the originally rewarding stimulus on average), as opposed to asymmetrical generalization (where there is a tendency to choose exaggerated versions of the initially rewarded stimulus, i.e.…”
Section: Discriminative and Generalized Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…When matching stimuli were placed farther apart or when the background stimulus was changed from the initial experimental conditions (from an uneven wooden surface with beams to a flat gray panel), horses could generalize information to these novel environments and presentations (Flannery, 1997). Dougherty and Lewis (1991),asked horses to discriminate between a black circle 2.5 inches in diameter (the positive or rewarded stimulus) and a black circle 1.5 inches in diameter (the negative stimulus). Interestingly, when horses were presented with intermediate stimuli Dougherty and Lewis (1991) found that, unlike pigeons and humans, horses showed symmetrical generalization gradients (they chose stimuli that were closest in size to the originally rewarding stimulus on average), as opposed to asymmetrical generalization (where there is a tendency to choose exaggerated versions of the initially rewarded stimulus, i.e.…”
Section: Discriminative and Generalized Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dougherty and Lewis (1991),asked horses to discriminate between a black circle 2.5 inches in diameter (the positive or rewarded stimulus) and a black circle 1.5 inches in diameter (the negative stimulus). Interestingly, when horses were presented with intermediate stimuli Dougherty and Lewis (1991) found that, unlike pigeons and humans, horses showed symmetrical generalization gradients (they chose stimuli that were closest in size to the originally rewarding stimulus on average), as opposed to asymmetrical generalization (where there is a tendency to choose exaggerated versions of the initially rewarded stimulus, i.e. choosing circles larger than the original positive stimulus on average, representing an avoidance of the negative stimulus).…”
Section: Discriminative and Generalized Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A compound case would be changing the size of the signal in s + , involving thus both a change of total intensity and different sets of units involved by different test stimuli. This latter case will not be investigated in this paper (but see Grice & Saltz, 1950;Mednick & Freedman, 1960;Dougherty & Lewis, 1991, for experimental data).…”
Section: Testing the Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Horses tend to be easy to study because even well-fed horses respond well to food reinforcement and studies indicate that horses can discriminate between people, shapes (34) , and colors (35) . Furthermore, horses show response matching (36) , some degree of stimulus generalization (37) , respond to different reinforcement schedules (38) and differential rewards (39) .…”
Section: Horsesmentioning
confidence: 99%