2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Speed discrimination in 6- and 10-month-old infants follows Weber’s law

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
19
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
4
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Across multiple literatures, there is evidence for many other contents that rely on continuous representations similar to the ANS; these include representations of approximate area (Brannon, Lutz, & Cordes, 2006;Odic, Libertus, et al, 2013;Odic, Pietroski, Hunter, Lidz, & Halberda, 2013), length (Droit-Volet et al, 2008), time (Droit-Volet et al, 2008;Meck & Church, 1983;Walsh, 2003), speed (Möhring, Libertus, & Bertin, 2012), and many more (Cantlon et al, 2009;Dehaene & Brannon, 2011;Feigenson, 2007). Each of these is available prelinguistically and humans in many cultures eventually master an ability to map from these representations into discrete values and vice versa (e.g., ''the speed limit is 130 km/h'', ''these crayons are various shades of red'').…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Across multiple literatures, there is evidence for many other contents that rely on continuous representations similar to the ANS; these include representations of approximate area (Brannon, Lutz, & Cordes, 2006;Odic, Libertus, et al, 2013;Odic, Pietroski, Hunter, Lidz, & Halberda, 2013), length (Droit-Volet et al, 2008), time (Droit-Volet et al, 2008;Meck & Church, 1983;Walsh, 2003), speed (Möhring, Libertus, & Bertin, 2012), and many more (Cantlon et al, 2009;Dehaene & Brannon, 2011;Feigenson, 2007). Each of these is available prelinguistically and humans in many cultures eventually master an ability to map from these representations into discrete values and vice versa (e.g., ''the speed limit is 130 km/h'', ''these crayons are various shades of red'').…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, young 3-year-olds have been shown to learn the meaning of the word more in context of both approximating number and approximating area, suggesting an underlying similarity between these two dimensions (Odic, Pietroski, Hunter, Lidz, & Halberda, 2012). Finally, in human infants, 6-month-olds have been shown to discriminate a ratio of 2.0 for the area of an Elmo face (Brannon, Lutz, & Cordes, 2006), and also to discriminate the same 2.0 ratio when comparing the number of elements in an array (Xu & Spelke, 2000), the duration of an auditory event (Brannon, Suanda, & Libertus, 2007; VanMarle & Wynn, 2006), and the speed of an object’s motion (Möhring, Libertus, & Bertin, 2012). However, not all quantity dimensions elicit the same acuity in 6-month-olds.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Looking time studies revealed that 5-month-old infants are sensitive to changes in metric distances (Newcombe, Huttenlocher, & Learmonth, 1999;Newcombe, Sluzenski & Huttenlocher, 2005), and toddlers encode distance metrically in a hide-and-seek game (Huttenlocher, Newcombe, & Sandberg, 1994). Furthermore, magnitude coding is used early in life, as evidenced by infants' discrimination of space, time, number, and speed (Brannon, Lutz, & Cordes, 2006;Brannon, Suanda, & Libertus, 2007;Möhring, Libertus, & Bertin, 2012;Xu & Spelke, 2000), and recent studies yielded evidence for cross-dimensional transfer, suggesting that magnitude information regarding various dimensions is coded in one representational system (de Hevia & Spelke, 2010;Lourenco & Longo, 2010). It is likely that metric understanding is based on this fundamental comparative system, termed the general magnitude system (Walsh, 2003).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%