2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.09.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Specifying the role of function in infant categorization

Abstract: Research demonstrates that object functions facilitate artifact categorization in infancy. To explicate the nature and magnitude of this effect, 16-month-olds participated in three studies. In Experiment 1, categorization was facilitated more by object functions than by distinctive motions, suggesting that the motion properties of function cannot fully explain its influence. In Experiment 2, infants failed to categorize when each category exemplar performed a different function, thus revealing the importance o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…1). Finally, some authors propose that, from 4 months, knowledge about object functions facilitates the categorical representation of artefacts (Stavans & Baillargeon, 2018 ; see Futó et al, 2010 ; Horst et al, 2005 with 10-month-olds; Booth & Waxman, 2002 ; Booth et al, 2010 with 14-month-old children). This evidence supports the possibility that first concepts are not motivated solely by perceptual similarities.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…1). Finally, some authors propose that, from 4 months, knowledge about object functions facilitates the categorical representation of artefacts (Stavans & Baillargeon, 2018 ; see Futó et al, 2010 ; Horst et al, 2005 with 10-month-olds; Booth & Waxman, 2002 ; Booth et al, 2010 with 14-month-old children). This evidence supports the possibility that first concepts are not motivated solely by perceptual similarities.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…4 Sample/subsample sizes. Densities and means for each distribution 1987), (ii) whether concepts are based on perceptual (e.g., Fagan, 1976;Welder & Graham, 2006) or functional object attributes (e.g., Booth et al, 2010;Nelson, 1973), (iii) whether basic-level (e.g., Mervis & Crisafi, 1982) or global-level categories (e.g., Mandler & McDonough, 1993;Rostad et al, 2012;Quinn, 2004) are developed first, (iv) whether the structure of concepts resembles that of a complex scientific theory (e.g., Gopnik, 1984) or is formed by everyday particular encounter with objects (e.g., Tomasello, 1999), (v) whether conceptual knowledge is organised in partonomies rather than taxonomies (e.g., Tversky & Hemenway, 1984) and (vi) whether language is essential to concept formation (e.g., Ferry et al, 2010;Xu, 1998) 3 . This links to the debate on the value of empirical indicators considered in each methodological perspective.…”
Section: Discussion: Methodological Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, 11-monthold infants are able to categorize different animal figures (e.g., a giraffe and a crocodile) and different furniture figures (e.g., a chair and a bed) as two contrasting and global kinds (Pauen, 2002), even though the degree of similarity between kinds was lower than the degree of similarity within kinds (e.g., Mandler & McDonough 1993;1998). Infants this same age have also been shown to categorize artifacts based on relevant functional information rather than their overall perceptual similarity (Trauble & Pauen, 2007), but only when object and function are causally connected (Booth, 2008;Booth, Schuler, & Zajicek, 2010;Gopnik & Sobel, 2000). In this way, infants, like adults, seem to represent function as a critical property of artifact kinds (Hernik & Csibra, 2009), and use kind membership as a more reliable source of information to categorize animals than perceptual similarity.…”
Section: Kind-based Categorizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In each case, the link between the artifact's outward physical structure and function could not easily be deduced, and the finding that infants assigned the lamp and the radio to separate categories (and hence succeeded at the task) only when given ostensive‐communicative signals was most likely due to this causal opacity. As in the work of Futó et al., much of the research on infants’ ability to form function‐based categories has used causally opaque artifacts with infants age 10 months and older (e.g., Baumgartner & Oakes, ; Booth, Schuler, & Zajicek, ; Booth & Waxman, ; Hernik & Csibra, ; Oakes & Madole, ). In this research, pedagogical signals have also been found to bolster infants’ encoding of functional information, by conveying that what may seem an arbitrary association (e.g., pulling a lever and lights flashing) actually represents an enduring causal relation that can be exploited in future actions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%