1986
DOI: 10.1044/jshr.2904.434
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Speaking Clearly for the Hard of Hearing II

Abstract: The first paper of this series (Picheny, Durlach, & Braida, 1985) presented evidence that there are substantial intelligibility differences for hearing-impaired listeners between nonsense sentences spoken in a conversational manner and spoken with the effort to produce clear speech. In this paper, we report the results of acoustic analyses performed on the conversational and clear speech. Among these results are the following. First, speaking rate decreases substantially in clear speech. This decrease is a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
70
2
5

Year Published

1999
1999
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 534 publications
(80 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
3
70
2
5
Order By: Relevance
“…A related question focuses on the relation between adaptive changes to production in a specific environment (such as the present experiment) and speech registers, such as clear speech, infant-, and foreigner-directed speech (e.g., Kuhl et al, 1997; Picheny et al, 1986; Uther et al, 2007). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A related question focuses on the relation between adaptive changes to production in a specific environment (such as the present experiment) and speech registers, such as clear speech, infant-, and foreigner-directed speech (e.g., Kuhl et al, 1997; Picheny et al, 1986; Uther et al, 2007). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, speech directed at adults differs systematically from speech directed at infants (e.g., Kuhl et al, 1997; Pate & Goldwater, 2015) or pets (e.g., Burnham, Kitamura, & Vollmer-Conna, 2002). Similarly, speech directed at typical adult native interlocutors differs from speech directed at non-native interlocutors (e.g., Uther, Knoll, & Burnham, 2007) or audiences with impaired comprehension (e.g., “clear speech”, speech directed at the hard of hearing, Picheny, Durlach, & Braida, 1986). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In human communication more generally, the phonetic properties of speech have been shown to vary with respect to communicational and situational demands (Picheny, Durlach, and Braida 1986). Lindblom (1990) describes principles governing these phonetic adaptations in terms of trade-offs: ''hyperarticulated'' speech is used to facilitate perception in contexts in which communication is harder or more important (e.g., slow and clear speech in a loud environment); otherwise, when perceptual demands are less severe, speech defaults to an articu-S Q U I B S A N D D I S C U S S I O N latorily easier form.…”
Section: What Does It Mean To Bear Meaning?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although clear speech is commonly produced with increased loudness, as well as a slower speaking rate (e.g., Ferguson & Quené, 2014;Lam et al, 2012;Picheny, Durlach, & Braida, 1986;Tjaden et al, 2013), the magnitude of change is much smaller in clear speech. In the current study, the relative change in vocal intensity in response to clear speech was similar to previously observed changes in vocal intensity during this speech modification .…”
Section: Task Performancementioning
confidence: 99%