2013
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00334
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial Working Memory Deficits Represent a Core Challenge for Rehabilitating Neglect

Abstract: Left neglect following right hemisphere injury is a debilitating disorder that has proven extremely difficult to rehabilitate. Traditional models of neglect have focused on impaired spatial attention as the core deficit and as such, most rehabilitation methods have tried to improve attentional processes. However, many of these techniques (e.g., visual scanning training, caloric stimulation, neck muscle vibration) produce only short-lived effects, or are too uncomfortable to use as a routine treatment. More rec… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 101 publications
(211 reference statements)
0
15
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, patients in the BIR Program received 30-minute daily computer-assisted cognitive training on RehaCom (Hasomed, Magdeburg, Germany) and CogniPlus (Schuhfried, Moedling, Austria) platforms. These cognitive training sessions were designed to target the core cognitive deficits in domains such as spatial attention and spatial working memory through computerized optokinetic stimulation [ 62 ] or training for spatial working memory (for discussion of association between spatial neglect and spatial working memory, see [ 65 ]). In addition, twice daily physiotherapy sessions in the BIR Program usually involved limb activation (with the help of physiotherapist and/or robotic systems) which was found to reduce spatial neglect even when performed passively [ 66 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, patients in the BIR Program received 30-minute daily computer-assisted cognitive training on RehaCom (Hasomed, Magdeburg, Germany) and CogniPlus (Schuhfried, Moedling, Austria) platforms. These cognitive training sessions were designed to target the core cognitive deficits in domains such as spatial attention and spatial working memory through computerized optokinetic stimulation [ 62 ] or training for spatial working memory (for discussion of association between spatial neglect and spatial working memory, see [ 65 ]). In addition, twice daily physiotherapy sessions in the BIR Program usually involved limb activation (with the help of physiotherapist and/or robotic systems) which was found to reduce spatial neglect even when performed passively [ 66 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For visuo-spatial working memory, tested with a delayed match-to sample design, it yielded a bilateral decrease in activation (Crottaz-Herbette et al, 2014) within regions known to be specifically involved in this task (Ackerman and Courtney, 2012;Awh et al, 1995;Chang et al, 2007;Courtney et al, 1998;Crottaz-Herbette et al, 2004;Sala and Courtney, 2007). This is of interest, because the spatial working memory network was shown to be coextensive with the dorsal attentional network, which sustains voluntary attentional orienting LaBar et al, 1999) and is often impaired in neglect (Husain et al, 2001;Striemer et al, 2013). Comparing fMRI activation patterns yielded by a target detection task before and after a single brief session of prismatic adaptation in 15 neglect patients highlighted a very similar PA-related modulation (Fig.…”
Section: Target Detection: Rightward Prismatic Adaptation Induces a Smentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is especially important when attempting to understand everyday functional deficits post-stroke, where the difficulties faced by neglect patients in representing their environment, for example, are due to poor spatiotemporal representations. The universality between the deployment of attention over a time course, and in space, may very well-explain why attentional deployment becomes gradually prolonged from non-neglected to neglected space (Di Pellegrino et al, 1998 ; Hillstrom et al, 2004 ), as well as why spatial working memory deficits are known to have a temporal component (i.e., due to the inability to keep track of spatial information over the course of time; Ferber and Danckert, 2006 ; Striemer et al, 2013 ). In fact, it could be the case that the temporal deployment of attention is an underlying factor that drives neglect behavior, such that when deployment speed becomes slow, it reaches a threshold where spatial symptoms of neglect begin to manifest.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%