1993
DOI: 10.3758/bf03205194
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial context affects the Poggendorff illusion

Abstract: The Poggendorffillusion has often been explained as purely an interaction between the parallels and the transversals. The present study demonstrates that additional spatial context exerts an influence on this illusion. In Experiment 1, we examined the effects of a surrounding tilted frame (complete and degraded versions) on collinearity adjustments in the upright and rotated Poggendorff figures. The frame's orientation was always oblique. Relative to the no-frame condition, frames decreased error in collineari… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the haptic experiments, subjects had to adjust the shape of the visual display to match the perceived shape, and in the analysis, we assumed that the visually generated shape was a veridical match to the one that was perceived, i.e., that no visual illusions were present [10], [13], [14]. We also assumed that inaccuracies in judging offsets visually were minimal.…”
Section: Control Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the haptic experiments, subjects had to adjust the shape of the visual display to match the perceived shape, and in the analysis, we assumed that the visually generated shape was a veridical match to the one that was perceived, i.e., that no visual illusions were present [10], [13], [14]. We also assumed that inaccuracies in judging offsets visually were minimal.…”
Section: Control Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Mu« ller-Lyer illusion is the most definitive example, where the direction of the tails of the figure, either feathering (making the line look longer) or arrowhead (making the line look shorter), influence perceived length. A number of other experimental examinations support the influence of visual context in creating visual illusions (Ku« nnapas 1955;Schiffman and Thompson 1978;Spivey-Knowlton and Bridgeman 1993). However, illusory effects are not limited to the situation where aspects within the same sensory system induce the illusion (ie visual context and a visual stimulus).…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Whatever the underlying mechanism(s) may be, the Poggendorff illusion depends on the spatial context within which it is viewed (Spivey- Knowlton & Bridgeman, 1993;Prinzmetal & Beck, 2001). Because the dusion is bound to diminish or even disappear when the figure is tilted so that the interrupting lines intersect vertically (at 90") or horizontally (at 180°), it is likely that a person's spatial visuhzation skills, such as the ab~lity to rotate complex figures mentally, are among the predictors of the amount of &stortion.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%