1995
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1995.81.2.531
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial Concepts and Perception of Physical and Diagrammed Scenes

Abstract: The accessibility of objects in mental spatial frameworks depends on their relation to the spatial axes of the world and people's typical interactions with space. The current study investigated perception of space. Subjects viewed either a physical model of a person surrounded by objects (Exp. 1) or diagrams of scenes (Exp. 2). Subjects named objects at directions from their own external perspective. For physical scenes, subjects were faster to name objects at Above/Below locations, followed by Front/Behind lo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
15
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
1
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our conclusion is based on the model presented by Franklin and Tversky (1990); (see also Bryant & Tversky, 1999), which postulates that spatial information is encoded within a reference system that is organized around extensions of the three major body axes. As in previous related studies not all differences between individual head/feet, front/back, and left/right comparisons were found to be significant (for some non‐significant patterns interpreted as imagined viewpoint switches, see Bryant & Wright, 1999; Bryant et al , 1992, 1995; Franklin & Tversky, 1990; Franklin et al , 1992). To the authors' knowledge the current findings are the first to suggest the adoption of an internal perspective after studying plan‐views in accordance with predictions from the spatial framework analysis.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our conclusion is based on the model presented by Franklin and Tversky (1990); (see also Bryant & Tversky, 1999), which postulates that spatial information is encoded within a reference system that is organized around extensions of the three major body axes. As in previous related studies not all differences between individual head/feet, front/back, and left/right comparisons were found to be significant (for some non‐significant patterns interpreted as imagined viewpoint switches, see Bryant & Wright, 1999; Bryant et al , 1992, 1995; Franklin & Tversky, 1990; Franklin et al , 1992). To the authors' knowledge the current findings are the first to suggest the adoption of an internal perspective after studying plan‐views in accordance with predictions from the spatial framework analysis.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…In other words, in past studies the general pattern has been considered along with inferential statistics to determine which imagined viewpoint was adopted by participants (e.g. Bryant, Lanca, & Tversky, 1995; Bryant et al , 1992; Bryant & Wright, 1999; Franklin & Tversky, 1990; Franklin, Tversky, & Coon, 1992). We adopt the same criterion in this investigation.…”
Section: The Spatial Framework Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, participants should be faster to respond to objects to front/back than head/feet, and slowest to objects to left/right. These predictions have been upheld in more than a dozen experiments (Bryant, Lanca, & Tversky, 1995;Bryant, Tversky, & Lanca, 1998;Franklin & Tversky, 1990;Franklin et al, 1992). 1 Participants construct similar mental models from experiencing a situation as from reading about a situation.…”
Section: Spatial Framework Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cognitive maps are mental representations that contain spatial information about locations, as well as the directional relationships and distances between them (Downs & Stea, 1977; Tolman, 1948). These mental representations are formed through perceptual experiences, for example, via locomotion and stationary viewing, through static pictorial representations, such as diagrams, paintings, and photos, and via dynamic pictorial representations, including animations, movies, and videos (Bryant, Lanca, & Tversky, 1995; Bryant & Tversky, 1999; Friedman & Montello, 2006; Richardson, Montello, & Hegarty, 1999; Thorndyke & Hayes‐Roth, 1982). Cognitive maps can also be formed through language, by means of descriptions of locations from the navigator’s perspective, descriptions of tours of the environment, and descriptions of locations using a fixed point of view such as wind directions (Taylor & Tversky, 1996).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%