2013
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00293
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spatial and temporal features of superordinate semantic processing studied with fMRI and EEG

Abstract: The relationships between the anatomical representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain and the timing of neurophysiological mechanisms involved in manipulating such information remain unclear. This is the case for superordinate semantic categorization—the extraction of general features shared by broad classes of exemplars (e.g., living vs. non-living semantic categories). We proposed that, because of the abstract nature of this information, input from diverse input modalities (visual or auditory, lex… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In general, participants showed an early positive peak near 150 ms (P1), a negative peak near 180 ms (N1), a middle positive peak near 240 ms (P2), a negative peak near 300 ms (N2), a positive peak near 350 ms (P3), a negative peak near 400 ms (N400), and a late positive peak near 510 ms (LPP). The amplitude greatly decreased to a negative amplitude after the late positive peak, resembling ERP waveforms reported in other studies (e.g., Costanzo and McArdle, 2013). The main effect of granularity was significant between 60–90 ms (β = 2.01, F = 10.03, p = 0.0016), 270–300 ms (β = -3.86, F = 20.43, p < 0.0001), and 540–570 ms (β = 3.29, F = 10.72; p = 0.0011), when we used alpha level 0.0025 (Bonferroni correction for 20 time bins).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 87%
“…In general, participants showed an early positive peak near 150 ms (P1), a negative peak near 180 ms (N1), a middle positive peak near 240 ms (P2), a negative peak near 300 ms (N2), a positive peak near 350 ms (P3), a negative peak near 400 ms (N400), and a late positive peak near 510 ms (LPP). The amplitude greatly decreased to a negative amplitude after the late positive peak, resembling ERP waveforms reported in other studies (e.g., Costanzo and McArdle, 2013). The main effect of granularity was significant between 60–90 ms (β = 2.01, F = 10.03, p = 0.0016), 270–300 ms (β = -3.86, F = 20.43, p < 0.0001), and 540–570 ms (β = 3.29, F = 10.72; p = 0.0011), when we used alpha level 0.0025 (Bonferroni correction for 20 time bins).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 87%
“…The images were divided into 13 categories: sporting equipment, shoes, electronics, kitchen supplies, office supplies, instruments, tools, clothing, faces, mammals, birds, bikes, and fruit. The categories could also be divided into subsets thought to be important in human semantic representation, such as living versus nonliving (Costanzo et al, 2013) or mobile versus stationary (Kriegeskorte et al, 2008). We compared our ''diffeomorphed'' images to three other image scrambling methods prevalent in the perception literature.…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Next to the EPN-counterpart in response to auditory emotional stimuli, evidence also suggests the existence of an equivalent to the late positive complex (LPC), which has reliably been shown to reflect sustained elaborate processing of emotional stimuli in the visual modality. An auditory LPC was reported for spoken words with emotional connotation (Ofek et al, 2013 ; Hatzidaki et al, 2015 ) and emotionally uttered words and sentences (Costanzo et al, 2013 ; Paulmann et al, 2013 ). Although there is evidence for some similarities between emotion-related ERP effects in the visual and auditory modality, it is noteworthy that these effects show pronounced differences in their temporal dynamics.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%