1973
DOI: 10.1177/096032717300500205
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Spaciousness in interiors

Abstract: Since traditional criteria for window design, e.g. the daylight factor, have lost their force, interest in re-evaluating the function of windows has been growing rapidly. The concept of spaciousness as an alternative criterion is introduced, and observations in scale models differing in spaciousness under an artificial sky have been carried out. From these, spaciousness for different sky luminances has been shown to be a function of interior illuminance, room size and window size. Observations in full scale in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
12
1
3

Year Published

1975
1975
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
2
12
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Applied to visual volume perception of indoor spaces, this would mean that perceivers' judgments could be biased by one salient dimension of the enclosure. This hypothesis is consistent with architectural findings showing elongation biases in area judgments of rectangular indoor spaces (e.g., rooms: Inui & Miyata, 1973;Sadalla & Oxley, 1984) and outdoor spaces (e.g., streets: Gärling, 1970;Ishikawa, Okabe, Sadahiro, & Kakumoto, 1998). Typically, rectangular architectural spaces are judged to be larger than square spaces of equal area (Inui & Miyata, 1973;Sadalla & Oxley, 1984).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Applied to visual volume perception of indoor spaces, this would mean that perceivers' judgments could be biased by one salient dimension of the enclosure. This hypothesis is consistent with architectural findings showing elongation biases in area judgments of rectangular indoor spaces (e.g., rooms: Inui & Miyata, 1973;Sadalla & Oxley, 1984) and outdoor spaces (e.g., streets: Gärling, 1970;Ishikawa, Okabe, Sadahiro, & Kakumoto, 1998). Typically, rectangular architectural spaces are judged to be larger than square spaces of equal area (Inui & Miyata, 1973;Sadalla & Oxley, 1984).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…This hypothesis is consistent with architectural findings showing elongation biases in area judgments of rectangular indoor spaces (e.g., rooms: Inui & Miyata, 1973;Sadalla & Oxley, 1984) and outdoor spaces (e.g., streets: Gärling, 1970;Ishikawa, Okabe, Sadahiro, & Kakumoto, 1998). Typically, rectangular architectural spaces are judged to be larger than square spaces of equal area (Inui & Miyata, 1973;Sadalla & Oxley, 1984). In the case of urban streets presented on a computer screen, the greater the depth of the street scene, the more spacious it appeared (Ishikawa et al, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Most of the literature on preferred window size is based on office environments (Butler & Steuerwald, 1991;Inui & Miyata, 1973;Keighley, 1973aKeighley, , 1973bMarkus, 1967;Ne'eman & Hopkinson, 1970). Inui and Miyata (1973) developed a predictive model, finding that Japanese participants judged the spaciousness of a scale model of an office based on the average horizontal illuminance on the work plane, the room volume, and the visual angle of the window at the viewing position (expressed as a percentage of the view); the coefficients varied with the sky luminance (of a diffuse artificial sky). These equations showed good agreement to judgements of a real office.…”
Section: Appearance Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In still another type of approach Inui and Miyata (1973) examined the effect of a window upon a subjective variable which they termed "spaciousness". Defining "spaciousness" as the feeling of openness or enclosure produced by an interior, they claimed that it is primarily determined by a person's visual perception of a space.…”
Section: Spaciousnessmentioning
confidence: 99%