PsycEXTRA Dataset 1967
DOI: 10.1037/e611302012-147
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sources of information for responses to reading test items

Abstract: The study dicotomizes the source of information in responding to multiple-choice items on reading comprehension tests into "learnings

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

1970
1970
1993
1993

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 1 publication
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These data are presented in Table 3. Preston (1964), Weaver (1967), and Tuinman (1973-1974. Taken together, these studies show that subjects have been able to perform quite well in a passage-out condition on a variety of standardized tests of reading comprehension.…”
Section: Analysis and Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These data are presented in Table 3. Preston (1964), Weaver (1967), and Tuinman (1973-1974. Taken together, these studies show that subjects have been able to perform quite well in a passage-out condition on a variety of standardized tests of reading comprehension.…”
Section: Analysis and Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Preston (1964) found that college students were able to score significantly higher than chance on the Cooperative English Test when deprived of the passages to which the test questions were supposed to relate. Weaver and Bickley (1967) found that college sophomores deprived of reading passages correctly answered 67 percent as many comprehension questions as sophomores who read the passages. Tuinman (1973Tuinman ( -1974, studying performance by fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade subjects on five highly regarded and widely used reading tests, found that subjects deprived of the reading passages scored higher than chance on the comprehension questions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Various writers have noted that unless the passages and items are carefully written, it may be possible for examinees to answer correctly the questions without paying careful attention to the associated passages (e.g., Davis, 1964, p. 105). This contention has been supported, in part, by Preston (1962), Weaver andBickley (1967), andPyrczak (1972), all of whom administered standardized readingcomprehension items without the passages. Pyrczak, furthermore, investigated the sources of information and misinformation that examinees used in responding to questions when they did not have access to the passages.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This characteristic, known as passagedependency, has been measured in tests of reading comprehension for native speakers, but so far as we know, no previous study has applied this concept t o the reading comprehension section of a major test of (non-native) language mastery. Using reading comprehension tests for native speakers of English, at least four studies have administered test items without the associated passages, and have concluded that not all items are highly passagedependent (Preston 1962, Weaver and Bickley 1967, Pyrczak 1972, and Tuinman 1973. Both Pyrczak (1972) and Tuinman (1973-74) have proposed ways of computing passagedependency indices.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%