2008
DOI: 10.1080/02699200701875864
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sonority and cross‐linguistic acquisition of initial s‐clusters

Abstract: This paper examines the findings and implications of the cross-linguistic acquisition of #sC clusters in relation to sonority patterns. Data from individual studies on English, Dutch, Norwegian, and Hebrew are compared for accuracy of production as well as the reductions with respect to potential differences across subtypes of #sC groups. In all four languages, a great deal of variability occurred both within and across children, but a number of general patterns were noted. While all four languages showed simi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

2
10
3

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
2
10
3
Order By: Relevance
“…While this goes against the principle of 'retain the least sonorous consonant' (so as to create a higher jump in sonority from the retained consonant to the vowel nucleus), all languages agreed in this violation. This, incidentally, reconfirms the patterns we found earlier with typically-developing children (Yavaş, Ben-David, Gerrits, Kristoffersen, and Simonsen, 2006), and questions the sonority index for nasal consonants. In addition, Spanish-English bilingual children also violated the above-mentioned principle by favouring the retention of [l] in the reductions of /sl/ targets, although /s/ is less sonorous than the lateral.…”
supporting
confidence: 89%
“…While this goes against the principle of 'retain the least sonorous consonant' (so as to create a higher jump in sonority from the retained consonant to the vowel nucleus), all languages agreed in this violation. This, incidentally, reconfirms the patterns we found earlier with typically-developing children (Yavaş, Ben-David, Gerrits, Kristoffersen, and Simonsen, 2006), and questions the sonority index for nasal consonants. In addition, Spanish-English bilingual children also violated the above-mentioned principle by favouring the retention of [l] in the reductions of /sl/ targets, although /s/ is less sonorous than the lateral.…”
supporting
confidence: 89%
“…Since in both nasals and stops the airflow is obstructed, this maybe motivated the creation of an independent group sharing the [-continuant] feature (Yavaş et al, 2008). The data in this study did not provide enough evidence to support neither.…”
Section: Results From #Sc Clusters Groupscontrasting
confidence: 52%
“…Both sonority distances 2 and -2 are within 2 steps difference on the sonority scale but in the opposite directions. Similar behavior was also observed in L1 studies (e.g., Gierut, 1999;Yavaş & Someillan, 2005;Yavaş, Ben-David, Gerrits, Kristoffersen & Simonsen, 2008). Gierut (1999) suggested that participants may be treating all sonority distances of 2 as a "natural class", regardless of the direction in which sonority heading, either "rising" as in /s+nasal/ or "falling" as in /s+stop/.…”
Section: Results From #Sc Clusters Groupsmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Yavaş, Ben David, Gerrits, Kristoffersen, & Simonsen, 2008). Those clusters (such as /sn/ in /snəʊ/) tend to be reduced to either the first or second consonant (i.e.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%