2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sometimes tool use is not the key: no evidence for cognitive adaptive specializations in tool-using woodpecker finches

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
41
3

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
3
41
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Problem-solving tasks, especially those that involve the removal of obstacles blocking access to food, have proved useful for this [22]. It was both Darwin's finch innovativeness in the wild and their strong performance in problem-solving tasks [23][24][25][26][27] that led Tebbich et al [11] to apply the flexible stem hypothesis to this clade. If our innovation data suggest that the stem is at the level of the family and superfamily, we should be able to identify other innovative Thraupidae and New World nine-primaried oscines that also show enhanced problem-solving abilities.…”
Section: A Review Of Innovativeness and Problemsolving In Loxigilla Bmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Problem-solving tasks, especially those that involve the removal of obstacles blocking access to food, have proved useful for this [22]. It was both Darwin's finch innovativeness in the wild and their strong performance in problem-solving tasks [23][24][25][26][27] that led Tebbich et al [11] to apply the flexible stem hypothesis to this clade. If our innovation data suggest that the stem is at the level of the family and superfamily, we should be able to identify other innovative Thraupidae and New World nine-primaried oscines that also show enhanced problem-solving abilities.…”
Section: A Review Of Innovativeness and Problemsolving In Loxigilla Bmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While primates have formed a significant focus of research into tool use, the same effect of more frequent and diverse tool use in captivity has been noted in animals as varied as elephants [31], keas [32], small tree finches [33], rooks [34], cockatoos [35], rodents such as degus [36] and naked mole rats [37] and even gastropods [38]. The examples listed here are illustrative rather than exhaustive-of the 418 instances of animal tool use tabulated by Bentley-Condit & Smith [4], 120 or 28.7% were found in captivity alone.…”
Section: 'Captivity Bias' In Animal Tool Use (A) Animal Tool Use In Cmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Where changes are observed in hominin behaviours that may be expected to influence tool use, such as ranging or diet, without accompanying changes in stone tool forms, we may therefore hypothesise that either (i) additional (non-lithic) tools were being incorporated into the toolkit, or (ii) existing tools were being sequentially combined for greater energy returns or to open up new extraction niches. Further, the captivity bias effect suggests that hominin groups, even if members of the same species, will show widely varying levels of tool use as a result of both cultural and stochastic processes [33]. Cognition is irrelevant to these processes.…”
Section: Hominins As Tool-using Animalsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This distinguishes innovativeness from more specialized cognitive abilities like food hoarding or song learning, which are easier to understand as a result of exposure to consistent selection pressures. Indeed, experimental work has failed to identify any cognitive specialization associated with enhanced innovative abilities [10,11]. Instead, some animals seem to possess from the start the machinery needed to invent sophisticated behaviours even when these are rarely used in the wild [12].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%