2009
DOI: 10.1075/avt.26.02ban
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Some implicatures reveal semantic differences

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While the results of Rubio-Fernandez and colleagues (2017) support their theory that comparison and categorization statements are scalar expressions, the results of their three sentence-verification experiments differ markedly from other scalar implicature studies testing "some" and "all" (for example, Banga et al, 2009;Bott & Noveck, 2004;Bott et al, 2012;Chemla & Bott, 2014;Cremers & Chemla, 2014;Degen & Goodman, 2014;De Neys & Schaeken, 2007;Feeney et al, 2004;Guasti et al, 2005;Hunt et al, 2013;Noveck, 2001;Noveck & Posada, 2003;Politzer-Ahles et al, 2013;Pouscoulous et al, 2007;Tavano & Kaiser, 2010;Tomlinson et al, 2013;Spychalska et al, 2016;van Tiel & Schnaeken, 2017;van Tiel et al, 2019). Specifically, participants in Rubio-Fernandez et al (2017) accepted underinformative comparisons as TRUE (e.g., "This one is like a tiger" predicated of a tiger) at considerably higher rates than those observed for underinformative statements with "some" (e.g., "Some elephants have trunks") in other studies using sentence verification (see Figure 1 for a summary illustration).…”
mentioning
confidence: 84%
“…While the results of Rubio-Fernandez and colleagues (2017) support their theory that comparison and categorization statements are scalar expressions, the results of their three sentence-verification experiments differ markedly from other scalar implicature studies testing "some" and "all" (for example, Banga et al, 2009;Bott & Noveck, 2004;Bott et al, 2012;Chemla & Bott, 2014;Cremers & Chemla, 2014;Degen & Goodman, 2014;De Neys & Schaeken, 2007;Feeney et al, 2004;Guasti et al, 2005;Hunt et al, 2013;Noveck, 2001;Noveck & Posada, 2003;Politzer-Ahles et al, 2013;Pouscoulous et al, 2007;Tavano & Kaiser, 2010;Tomlinson et al, 2013;Spychalska et al, 2016;van Tiel & Schnaeken, 2017;van Tiel et al, 2019). Specifically, participants in Rubio-Fernandez et al (2017) accepted underinformative comparisons as TRUE (e.g., "This one is like a tiger" predicated of a tiger) at considerably higher rates than those observed for underinformative statements with "some" (e.g., "Some elephants have trunks") in other studies using sentence verification (see Figure 1 for a summary illustration).…”
mentioning
confidence: 84%
“…The implicature control items consisted of two sentence types with the scalar expressions sommige “some 1 ” (6) or enkele “some 2 ” (7) (24 items per sentence type). In contrast to enkele , which merely expresses existential quantification (“there are some…”), sommige additionally indicates that the individuals introduced by the quantifier have something in common that distinguishes them from other individuals (de Hoop and Kas, 1989 ; Banga et al, 2009 ). For each item, a different intransitive verb was used (e.g., fishing, dancing, singing, and sleeping).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…. "), sommige additionally indicates that the individuals introduced by the quantifier have something in common that distinguishes them from other individuals (de Hoop and Kas, 1989;Banga et al, 2009). For each item, a different intransitive verb was used (e.g., fishing, dancing, singing, and sleeping).…”
Section: Implicature Control Itemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1 tested the partitive 'some of the' instead of the bare form 'enkele' (= 'some') that was tested by Geurts and Pouscoulous. It has been observed that partitives lead to higher rates of UBCs than bare forms (e.g., Banga et al, 2009).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%