2014
DOI: 10.1017/s0954394514000155
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Socially-mediated syntactic alignment

Abstract: A B S T R A C TWhen we interact with one another, we tend to align our behaviors, including the way we talk. Psycholinguistic work has conceptualized alignment as the result of automatic cognitive mechanisms that operate to facilitate processing and communication. Sociolinguistic work has focused on the role of social identity and interactional strategy in explaining linguistic alignment. We draw on these two largely distinct traditions to investigate socially mediated syntactic alignment with the goal of unde… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
63
1
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 79 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
3
63
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Previous research on pronunciation variation argues that speakers adapt their pronunciations as a function of alignment with interlocutors (e.g., Babel, 2010; Giles et al, 1991; Hay, 2000; Sanchez, Hay, & Nilson, 2015; possibly partly with social goals; for discussion, see Campbell-Kibler, 2010; Foulkes & Hay, 2015; Weatherholtz et al, 2014) or due to, for example, competition processes during lexical planning (Arnold et al, 2012; Baese-Berk & Goldrick, 2009; Kahn & Arnold, 2012; Watson, Buxó-Lugo, & Simmons, 2015; for discussion, see Buz & Jaeger, 2015; Jaeger & Buz, in press). The present results and the studies discussed in the previous section show that feedback from interlocutors also influences pronunciation variation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Previous research on pronunciation variation argues that speakers adapt their pronunciations as a function of alignment with interlocutors (e.g., Babel, 2010; Giles et al, 1991; Hay, 2000; Sanchez, Hay, & Nilson, 2015; possibly partly with social goals; for discussion, see Campbell-Kibler, 2010; Foulkes & Hay, 2015; Weatherholtz et al, 2014) or due to, for example, competition processes during lexical planning (Arnold et al, 2012; Baese-Berk & Goldrick, 2009; Kahn & Arnold, 2012; Watson, Buxó-Lugo, & Simmons, 2015; for discussion, see Buz & Jaeger, 2015; Jaeger & Buz, in press). The present results and the studies discussed in the previous section show that feedback from interlocutors also influences pronunciation variation.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A short demonstration of the paradigm is available at https://www.hlp.rochester.edu/mturk/demos/simpartnerdemo/index.html.. Our lab has successfully used this web-based approach for spoken sentence elicitation (Jaeger & Grimshaw, 2013; Weatherholtz, Campbell-Kibler, & Jaeger, 2014) and speech perception (Kleinschmidt & Jaeger, 2012; Kleinschmidt, Raizada, & Jaeger, 2015). The present paper extends it to the study of phonetic production.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The scale was such that 1 referred to least human/least strange/lowest quality, whereas 6 referred to most human/most strange/highest quality. The second was 7 questions relating to their social opinion of the avatar (adapted from [24,27]; hereafter Interpersonal Distance Questionnaire ). These questions were phrased as statements (see Table 2 for a complete list) and the participants indicated the extent to which they agreed with each statement on a 6-point Likert scale (6 = I absolutely agree , 1 = I do not agree at all ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In 2014, Weatherholtz and colleagues [24] conducted a similar experiment, but instead of the binomial “mean” versus “nice” confederate they had participants complete a survey which measured how similar the participants found themselves to their partner, giving a wider spread of ratings. The experiment was conducted online, with participants first hearing a political diatribe after which they were asked to describe 10 simple line drawings.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Examining the makeup or “loadings” of these factors—here, the extent to which each of the original rating scales contributes to a given summary factor—can reveal the ways in which responses to these rating scales are correlated with one another. Further, the output of these factors (factor scores) for each participant can be used to assess how these summary factors may relate to the experimental manipulations tested(Weatherholz, Jaeger & Campbell‐Kibler, , serves as an example of EFA used in sociolinguistics).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%