2011
DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0881
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social status gates social attention in humans

Abstract: ).Humans tend to shift attention in response to the averted gaze of a face they are fixating, a phenomenon known as gaze cuing. In the present paper, we aimed to address whether the social status of the cuing face modulates this phenomenon. Participants were asked to look at the faces of 16 individuals and read fictive curriculum vitae associated with each of them that could describe the person as having a high or low social status. The association between each specific face and either high or low social statu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

26
167
5

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 159 publications
(198 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
26
167
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Detecting that you have caused an individual to re-orient their attention to align their attention with yours may be a socially rewarding experience [7,14], akin to a social approach [59] or detecting that one has been imitated [22]. We already know that macaque [60,61] and human [62,63] social attention is influenced by affiliation, perceived dominance or status of the other individual. One could speculate the converse relationship-that being followed could promote feelings of empowerment in the 'leader' as they exert control over others' behaviour (even incidentally).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Detecting that you have caused an individual to re-orient their attention to align their attention with yours may be a socially rewarding experience [7,14], akin to a social approach [59] or detecting that one has been imitated [22]. We already know that macaque [60,61] and human [62,63] social attention is influenced by affiliation, perceived dominance or status of the other individual. One could speculate the converse relationship-that being followed could promote feelings of empowerment in the 'leader' as they exert control over others' behaviour (even incidentally).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The gaze-cueing task is based on those used in previous studies [4,11]: first, a white cross appeared for 500 ms. Second, a face with direct gaze appeared for 900 ms, after which the pupils moved to the left or right for 200 or 800 ms, and then a white target ('T' or 'L') appeared with a time-out limit of 2000 ms. After the target, a black screen appeared for 2, 4, 6 or 8 s (figure 1).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The intergroup relations between the two groups were manipulated as threatening in study a, and as non-threatening in study b. Then a standard gaze-cueing paradigm was employed [4]. Our expectation was that participants would follow the gaze of threatening out-group faces in study a, while participants would not follow the gaze of non-threatening out-group faces in study b.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A full understanding of joint attention requires us to understand these top-down processes. For example, we are more likely to follow the gaze of certain individuals such as people who are of high status (Dalmaso, Pavan, Castelli, & Galfano, 2012), or of our own political persuasion (Liuzza et al, 2013) or race (Dalmaso, Galfano, & Castelli, 2015). Whilst several papers have reported gaze following to be independent of emotional expression (Bayliss, Frischen, Fenske, & Tipper, 2007;Hietanen & Leppanen, 2003;Holmes, Richards, & Green, 2006), it has become apparent that when participants were required to detect an emotionally salient target, the emotional expression does influence gaze cueing, with stronger cueing for fearful than for happy faces (Bayliss, Schuch, & Tipper, 2010;Kuhn & Tipples, 2011;Pecchinenda, Pes, Ferlazzo, & Zoccolotti, 2008), which suggests that joint attentional processes may be more strategic than previously thought (Baron-Cohen, 1995).…”
Section: Own-age Biases In Adults' and Children's Joint Attention: Bimentioning
confidence: 99%