1973
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1973.tb00103.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social schemas, cognitive complexity, and the learning of social structures1

Abstract: There is a striking parallelism between Heider's balance prmciple (1946, 1958) and Desoto's ordenng schema (DeSoto, i960, 1961, Henley, Horsfall, & DeSoto, 1969. At the general level, of course, the two principles sprmg from similar theoretical approaches Both positions are Cestaltist, as refiected m their concern for the pattern of relations among a set of elements, both are also phenomenological, m the sense that they are much more concemed with how perceivers construe the relationships among elements than… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
20
0

Year Published

1977
1977
1997
1997

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
3
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Presently, however, there are no valid conceptual or empirical reasons for believing that the RCQ measure of cognitive complexity is meaningfully confounded by general verbal abilities or proclivities. Indeed, the results of prior research (Applegate et al, 1985;Burleson et al, 1981;Burleson & Rowan, 1985;Crockett, 1965;Delia, 1978;Delia & Crockett, 1973;Hale, 1980;Press et al, 1969;O'Keefe & Sypher, 1981;Scarlett et al, 1971;Sypher & Applegate, 1982) as well as the results of the two studies reported here strongly suggest that the RCQ measure of cognitive complexity is unconfounded by general verbal traits or capacities. Future attacks on the RCQ must carefully address the extensive evidence supporting the reliability and validity of this measure.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 45%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Presently, however, there are no valid conceptual or empirical reasons for believing that the RCQ measure of cognitive complexity is meaningfully confounded by general verbal abilities or proclivities. Indeed, the results of prior research (Applegate et al, 1985;Burleson et al, 1981;Burleson & Rowan, 1985;Crockett, 1965;Delia, 1978;Delia & Crockett, 1973;Hale, 1980;Press et al, 1969;O'Keefe & Sypher, 1981;Scarlett et al, 1971;Sypher & Applegate, 1982) as well as the results of the two studies reported here strongly suggest that the RCQ measure of cognitive complexity is unconfounded by general verbal traits or capacities. Future attacks on the RCQ must carefully address the extensive evidence supporting the reliability and validity of this measure.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 45%
“…In particular, Crockett's RCQ procedure for assessing interpersonal cognitive complexity has repreatedly been shown free from such potentially confounding influences as verbal intelligence, verbal fluency, general intellectual abilities (IQ), grade point average, performance on standardized test such as the SAT, narrative writing skills, and writing speed (e.g. Applegate, Burke, Burleson, Delia, & Kline, 1985;Burleson et al, 1981;Burleson & Rowan, 1985;Crockett, 1965;Delia, 1978;Delia & Crockett, 1973;Hale, 1980;Press, Crockett, and Rosenkrantz, 1969;Scarlett, Press, & Crockett, 1971;Sypher & Applegate, 1982).…”
Section: Cognitive Complexity: Theory and Measurementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A further reason is suggested from the view ofJones and Nisbett (1972) that an actor-observer (analogous to P = S) may not act at the peak of cognitive complexity but is likely to use 'simplifying assumptions about the environment' (p. 87). It has been shown (Delia and Crockett, 1973;Press et al, 1969;Steinitz, 1969), that cognitively simple subjects tend to balance structures, and that structures forming part of a simple cognitive domain are more likely to be balanced than those forming part of a complex cognitive domain (Scott, 1963). Such studies strengthen the view that when P = S he may be acting as though cognitively simple and thus tend to form and prefer balanced triads.…”
Section: The Identification Of the Subject With A Member Of The Triadmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Burleson (1987) cited more than 40 studies that support this relationship. Other constructivist studies have shown that construct systems become more differentiated with age and are characterized by a change from concrete to abstract (Scarlett, Press, & Crockett, 1971), that females' construct systems are generally more complex than males' (Delia, Clark, & Switzer, 1974), that persons with complex systems are better able to integrate inconsistent information and behavior (Nidorf & Crockett, 1965), that cognitively complex persons are better able to take others' perspectives in social situations (Hale & Delia, 1976), that cognitively complex persons are not as dependent as cognitively simple persons on simplifying cognitive schemas when trying to understand social situations (Delia & Crockett, 1973), and that persons with more complex systems are more likely than those with less complex systems to be upwardly mobile in organizational settings (B. D. Sypher & Zorn, 1986).…”
Section: O'keefementioning
confidence: 99%