2015
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0934
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Social learning in a high-risk environment: incomplete disregard for the ‘minnow that cried pike’ results in culturally transmitted neophobia

Abstract: Many prey species rely on conspecifics to gather information about unknown predation threats, but little is known about the role of varying environmental conditions on the efficacy of social learning. We examined predator-naive minnows that had the opportunity to learn about predators from experienced models that were raised in either a low-or high-risk environment. There were striking differences in behaviour among models; high-risk models showed a weaker response to the predator cue and became neophobic in r… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
(63 reference statements)
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Such social learning has been noted in a variety of animal societies [17,18,[54][55][56][57] and has profound theoretical ramifications for how societies function [58]. In this study, we show that the effects of information quality on group performance differ based on the identity of its original bearer.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Such social learning has been noted in a variety of animal societies [17,18,[54][55][56][57] and has profound theoretical ramifications for how societies function [58]. In this study, we show that the effects of information quality on group performance differ based on the identity of its original bearer.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…When examining this finding, it is necessary to examine the necessary ingredients for socially learned avoidance to occur. Griffin () described the necessity of a “fear response” on the part of experienced individuals that naïve individuals could detect, and indeed specific anti‐predator behaviours (freezing, erratic swimming, the release of alarm cues) on the part of experienced individuals have been found to be key to learning as naïve individuals follow experienced individuals’ lead (Crane & Ferrari, ; Crane et al, ; Manassa & McCormick, , ). This process may not apply to the threat of angling, however, as experienced fish may avoid lures by simply ignoring them, without a pronounced fear response that is detectable by naïve observers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Being in the presence of experienced individuals also facilitated improved avoidance behaviour in naïve guppies escaping a model trawl net (Brown & Laland, ), and naïve fishes have been shown to learn more quickly to associate predator odours with danger when they were placed together with individuals experienced with that predator (i.e. through observational conditioning, Crane, Mathiron, & Ferrari, ; Manassa & McCormick, ). Social learning has also been implicated in the avoidance of capture by humans engaged in recreational fishing.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…larger versus smaller lesions or longer versus shorter social isolation), making cross-treatment comparisons difficult. A few studies have compared neophobic responses induced by risk versus responses towards known predator cues, finding either similar outcomes [22,67] or that neophobic responses were weaker [22,68]. Table 1.…”
Section: (F ) Inducing Treatment Typementioning
confidence: 99%