2012
DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2011.642795
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Slave systems in verbal short-term memory

Abstract: Background The model of performance in short-term memory (STM) tasks that has been most influential in cognitive neuropsychological work on deficits of STM is the “working memory” model mainly associated with the work of Alan Baddeley and his colleagues. Aim This paper reviews the model. We examine the development of this theory in studies that account for STM performances in normal (non-brain-damaged) individuals, and then review the application of this theory to neuropsychological cases and specifications,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
25
0
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 160 publications
(235 reference statements)
0
25
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Martin et al, 1994), for example when more than one noun precedes a verb. That verbal STM might involve not only phonological representations, but also lexical-semantic representations, suggests that a purely phonological store (as proposed by Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) may be too restrictive (see also Caplan, Waters, & Howard, 2012). Accordingly, language-based models of STM have emphasized the contribution of multiple levels of linguistic representations, including both phonological and lexical-semantic codes (e.g., N. Martin & Ayala, 2004; N.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Martin et al, 1994), for example when more than one noun precedes a verb. That verbal STM might involve not only phonological representations, but also lexical-semantic representations, suggests that a purely phonological store (as proposed by Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) may be too restrictive (see also Caplan, Waters, & Howard, 2012). Accordingly, language-based models of STM have emphasized the contribution of multiple levels of linguistic representations, including both phonological and lexical-semantic codes (e.g., N. Martin & Ayala, 2004; N.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, our key point is to argue that this cannot be inferred from the absence of measurable phonological similarity and word length effects (cf. Caplan et al, 2012). The novel advance that we are able to make here is to show that reduced verbal short-term memory capacity will itself necessarily lead to reduced, absent, or even reversed phonological similarity and word length effects due to proportional scaling coupled with floor effects.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given that patients with a verbal short-term memory deficit will, almost by definition, show very low levels of immediate serial recall, and if the size of these effects is proportional to overall performance, then one would expect similarity and length effects to be smaller than normal in such patients simply as a consequence of their memory impairment. Indeed, as Caplan et al (2012) note, when discussing the implications of a reduced phonological similarity effect (PSE) in the context of neuropsychological studies, Blower list lengths and reduced spans are associated with a smaller PSE^(p. 294). In addition, because overall immediate serial recall performance is known to be poorer when material is presented visually as opposed to auditorily (e.g., Harvey & Beaman, 2007;Murray, 1966) one would expect this reduction to be particularly marked with visual presentation (cf.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations