1991
DOI: 10.1364/josaa.8.000661
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Simultaneous color constancy: papers with diverse Munsell values

Abstract: Arend and Reeves [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 1743 (1986)] described measurements of color constancy in computer simulations of arrays of colored papers of equal Munsell value under 4000-, 6500-, and 10,000-K daylight illuminants. We report an extension of those experiments to chromatic arrays spanning a wide range of Munsell values. The computer-simulated scene included a standard array of Munsell papers under 6500-K illumination and a test array, an identical array of the same papers under 4000 or 10,000 K. Observ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
152
2

Year Published

2000
2000
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 191 publications
(163 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
(7 reference statements)
9
152
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet surface-colour matches can certainly be made with such scenes (Fig. 2a), and they do not seem much worse than with scenes containing many surfaces [21,22] (as in Fig. 1a, the illuminants on the left and right are different).…”
Section: Relative Versus Absolute Judgmentsmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Yet surface-colour matches can certainly be made with such scenes (Fig. 2a), and they do not seem much worse than with scenes containing many surfaces [21,22] (as in Fig. 1a, the illuminants on the left and right are different).…”
Section: Relative Versus Absolute Judgmentsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Instructions of this kind can be important, as subjects might concentrate on the colour of the light -hue, saturation, brightness -rather than on the colour of the surface [10,17,18,21 -23]. To make this distinction clearer, look at a surface half in shadow: the shadowed region has the same surface colour as the unshadowed region but it simultaneously appears less bright and generally more blue [22].…”
Section: Matching Surfacesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is not only introspectively verifiable-it is the standard view in the literature on colour perception '' (2006, p. 84). And he notes further that in colour constancy experiments [for instance, Arend, Reeves, Schirillo and Goldstein (1991)], subjects are able to match two coloured papers for hue, saturation, and brightness even when those same two papers do not look to the subject to have the same surface colour (Thompson 2006, p. 85). to assume that in either case your experience will misrepresent the colour of the paper-in fact, we know that the visual systems of normal humans are very good at determining the unchanging colour of a surface despite changes to its ''sensory appearance.'' 9 So, once again, we have two phenomenally identical experiences of different coloured objects and yet each experience represents the colour of the relevant object veridically.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…11 3 Thompson's solution So, the Fregean representationalist has a problem: she needs to add something to the relevant modes of presentation in order to accommodate colour constancy, but the most obvious strategy (including lighting conditions) won't work. A more 9 See, for example, Arend et al (1991). 10 See Thompson (2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%