2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Simulating visibility during language comprehension

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

8
77
0
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 99 publications
(86 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
8
77
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Zwaan, Stanfield, and Yaxley (2002), for example, found that after reading a sentence such as The ranger saw the eagle in the sky participants were faster to judge a picture of an eagle as mentioned versus not mentioned in the sentence when the depicted shape (wings outstretched) matched the shape implied in the sentence (the eagle is in the sky) compared to when it did not match (perched). Stanfield and Zwaan (2001) observed related findings concerning object orientation, while Yaxley and Zwaan (2005) provided evidence that readers simulate even the visibility of described scenes (e.g., as foggy versus clear). The above observations have generally been taken as support for the seamless interaction of visual and linguistic representations (Tanenhaus et al, 1995), on the one hand, and for multi-modal sensorimotor-derived meaning representations on the other (Barsalou, 1999b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Zwaan, Stanfield, and Yaxley (2002), for example, found that after reading a sentence such as The ranger saw the eagle in the sky participants were faster to judge a picture of an eagle as mentioned versus not mentioned in the sentence when the depicted shape (wings outstretched) matched the shape implied in the sentence (the eagle is in the sky) compared to when it did not match (perched). Stanfield and Zwaan (2001) observed related findings concerning object orientation, while Yaxley and Zwaan (2005) provided evidence that readers simulate even the visibility of described scenes (e.g., as foggy versus clear). The above observations have generally been taken as support for the seamless interaction of visual and linguistic representations (Tanenhaus et al, 1995), on the one hand, and for multi-modal sensorimotor-derived meaning representations on the other (Barsalou, 1999b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Recent embodied and simulation based approaches to language comprehension predict that comprehension of language triggers mental simulation of events and objects as described in the language [1][2][3][4]. According to theories of embodied cognition [5], such simulation depends upon the mental representations that were formed during comprehenders' actual perceptual experience and interaction with the environment.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Support for this idea is extensive, indicating that readers simulate multiple aspects of the characters' experience, such as their visual perspective, including the shape, orientation, and color of objects (Connell, 2007;Stanfield & Zwaan, 2001;Yaxley & Zwaan, 2007), as well as their motor movements (Speer, Reynolds, Swallow, & Zachs, 2009;Taylor, Lev-Ari, & Zwaan, 2008;. For example, Glenberg and Kaschak (2002) found that after reading a sentence describing a character making a movement in a particular direction (e.g., "He closed the drawer"), readers were faster to press a key on a response box that involved making a movement in the same direction as the character (e.g., moving their hand away from their body) than making a movement in the opposite direction.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%