1996
DOI: 10.1080/016909696387196
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sidestepping Garden Paths: Assessing the Contributions of Syntax, Semantics and Plausibility in Resolving Ambiguities

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
97
5
1

Year Published

1999
1999
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 122 publications
(112 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
6
97
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…First, the presence of the three-and-one-referent context allows for the conclusion that referential effects in syntactic ambiguity resolution reflect not only a highly incremental process of establishing reference, but also, more specifically, the on-line use of linguistically coded presuppositions. That is, it distinguishes between a strictly referential account, couched in terms of identification of a referent (e.g., Ni, Crain, & Shankweiler, 1996), and a more general pragmatic account in which expectations for modification affect ambiguity resolution. Second, and perhaps most important, the comparisons of the two-referent and one-referent contexts in and Trueswell et al (1999) were insufficient to rule out the possibility that the effect of a temporary garden path on eye movements was masked by inhibition from potential eye movements to an alternative referent.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, the presence of the three-and-one-referent context allows for the conclusion that referential effects in syntactic ambiguity resolution reflect not only a highly incremental process of establishing reference, but also, more specifically, the on-line use of linguistically coded presuppositions. That is, it distinguishes between a strictly referential account, couched in terms of identification of a referent (e.g., Ni, Crain, & Shankweiler, 1996), and a more general pragmatic account in which expectations for modification affect ambiguity resolution. Second, and perhaps most important, the comparisons of the two-referent and one-referent contexts in and Trueswell et al (1999) were insufficient to rule out the possibility that the effect of a temporary garden path on eye movements was masked by inhibition from potential eye movements to an alternative referent.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These sentences contrasting two types of reduced relative clause constructions, ones marked by the determine "the" versus ones marked by the focus operator "only" (Kemtes, 1998;Ni et al, 1996). The focus operator sets up an expectation of contrasting set, blocking the main verb interpretation, as in "Only businessmen loaned money at low interest rates…" Ni et al claim that this contrast is semantic or referential and biases the initial interpretation of the verb as a reduced relative clause.…”
Section: Experiments IImentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This elaboration should lead to a richer, more interconnected memory representation, making the anaphor a more effective retrieval cue to memory. Moreover, adjectives often serve a contrastive function and are used to distinguish between referents (Ni, Crain, & Shankweiler, 1996;Sedivy, Tanenhaus, Chambers, & Carlson, 1999). Thus, the prenominal adjectives in this experiment may serve as additional cues that there is a specific referent in the discourse model.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%