2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2020.101973
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Should we sort it out later? The effect of tracking age on long-run outcomes

Abstract: published version features the final layout of the paper including the volume, issue and page numbers. Link to publication General rightsCopyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights.• Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests that, for cognitive achievement, track allocation is efficient such that no marginal students would benefit from switching track. It may be surprising that higher peer quality in T H does not translate into achievement gains, but these findings are in line with other research for the Netherlands that identifies weak or no short-run achievement effects of track assignment or tracking age (Borghans et al, 2019(Borghans et al, , 2020.…”
Section: Cognitive Outcomessupporting
confidence: 85%
“…This suggests that, for cognitive achievement, track allocation is efficient such that no marginal students would benefit from switching track. It may be surprising that higher peer quality in T H does not translate into achievement gains, but these findings are in line with other research for the Netherlands that identifies weak or no short-run achievement effects of track assignment or tracking age (Borghans et al, 2019(Borghans et al, , 2020.…”
Section: Cognitive Outcomessupporting
confidence: 85%
“…This does not mean our study settles the matter: our design is not perfect either. The most important limitation is that whether children delay tracking is not exogenously controlled, which the non-genetic literature deals better with by using difference-in-difference 69 or instrumental variable approaches 26 . This leaves open the possibility that children that choose to delay tracking are somehow also the ones that would have been better in realizing their genetic potential if they would have been tracked immediately.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Van Elk et al 25 report for those with a MAVO track recommendation (old label for VMBO-t) that children from higher-SES and more urbanized areas more often attend a heterogeneous class, but that none of the effects are significant in a multivariate analysis. Borghans et al 26 show for those with a HAVO to VWO recommendation that higher-SES and higher-performing children are less likely to attend a heterogeneous class. All in all, the evidence does not point towards strong selectivity, but nevertheless we check whether two potential confounders, namely family SES and school performance of the child, influence the choice to delay tracking or not.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hanushek and Wössmann, 2006;Ruhose and Schwerdt, 2016;Matthewes, 2020). 54 Second, much of the remaining evidence comes from studies looking at the long-run effects of postponing the age of tracking (Meghir and Palme, 2005;Aakvik et al, 2010;Pekkala Kerr et al, 2013;Borghans et al, 2020;Canaan, 2020). Students are still tracked at some point during secondary school in these contexts.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%