2009
DOI: 10.1037/a0015360
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Shifting development in mid-childhood: The influence of between-task interference.

Abstract: Performance on the task-switching paradigm is greatly affected by the amount of conflict between tasks. Compared to adults, children appear to be particularly influenced by this conflict, suggesting that the ability to resolve interference between tasks improves with age. We used the task-switching paradigm to investigate how this ability develops in mid-childhood. Experiment 1 compared 5-to 8-year-olds' and 9-to 11-year-olds' ability to switch between decisions about the colour of an object and its shape. The… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
30
0
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
6
30
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…To this end, 4-and 5-year-olds were tested in two conditions differing in the degree of cue transparency, that is, the strength with which cues were associated with the goals they signaled. In the flexibility phase, performance was higher for color naming than for shape naming and, consistent with previous studies (e.g., Cragg & Nation, 2009;Ellefson, Shapiro, & Chater, 2006), switching yielded a higher cost in performance for the easier color-naming task than for the more difficult shape-naming task. More important for the current study, both age groups performed faster and more accurately on both switch and no-switch trials in this phase when demands on goal representation were alleviated by using transparent cues instead of arbitrary cues.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…To this end, 4-and 5-year-olds were tested in two conditions differing in the degree of cue transparency, that is, the strength with which cues were associated with the goals they signaled. In the flexibility phase, performance was higher for color naming than for shape naming and, consistent with previous studies (e.g., Cragg & Nation, 2009;Ellefson, Shapiro, & Chater, 2006), switching yielded a higher cost in performance for the easier color-naming task than for the more difficult shape-naming task. More important for the current study, both age groups performed faster and more accurately on both switch and no-switch trials in this phase when demands on goal representation were alleviated by using transparent cues instead of arbitrary cues.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Larger switch costs are found for the colour task, which participants find easier to perform (Cragg & Nation, 2009;Ellefson, Shapiro, & Chater, 2006). Interestingly, the DCCS also employs colour and shape tasks; however, no difference has been found in switching between the two tasks (e.g., Frye et al, 1995).…”
Section: Overcoming the Previously Irrelevant Taskmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In a version of the task-switching paradigm used with older children, separating responses was found to help 9-to 11-year-olds but not 5-to 7-year-olds switch task (Cragg & Nation, 2009, Experiment 2), perhaps indicating that the level at which conflict causes most difficulty may change with age. In contrast to the work with preschoolers, separating the stimulus dimensions by placing the shape on a coloured background increased stimulus-level conflict in this study, particularly for the 5-to 7-year-olds.…”
Section: Information Filteringmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations