2008
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-540-69132-7_66
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Shall We Explain? Augmenting Learning from Intelligent Tutoring Systems and Peer Collaboration

Abstract: Abstract. Learning outcomes from intelligent tutoring systems (ITSs) tend to be quite strong, usually in the neighborhood of one standard deviation. However, most ITS designers use the learning outcomes from expert human tutoring as the gold standard (i.e., two standard deviations). What can be done, with the current state of the art, to increase learning from an ITS? One method is to modify the learning situation by asking students to use the ITS in pairs. To enhance performance, we drew upon the beneficial e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
4
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Although self‐explaining may be a powerful constructive activity, it may not be as powerful as jointly explaining with a partner, an interactive activity. There is suggestive evidence that jointly explaining either with an expert (Chi et al., 2008) or with a peer (Hausmann & VanLehn, 2007) is more beneficial to learning than self‐explaining alone. However, self‐explaining, a constructive activity, might be better for learning when compared to an active activity such as selecting an explanation from a menu (Corbett, Wagner, Lesgold, Ulrich, & Stevens, 2006) .…”
Section: A Hypothesis Generated From This Taxonomy and Evidence Inmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although self‐explaining may be a powerful constructive activity, it may not be as powerful as jointly explaining with a partner, an interactive activity. There is suggestive evidence that jointly explaining either with an expert (Chi et al., 2008) or with a peer (Hausmann & VanLehn, 2007) is more beneficial to learning than self‐explaining alone. However, self‐explaining, a constructive activity, might be better for learning when compared to an active activity such as selecting an explanation from a menu (Corbett, Wagner, Lesgold, Ulrich, & Stevens, 2006) .…”
Section: A Hypothesis Generated From This Taxonomy and Evidence Inmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results consistently show better performance in comparison to active or passive modes alone. Then, Hausmann, van de Sande and VanLehn (2008) compared the constructive mode (self‐explaining individually) with the interactive mode (explaining done with a peer). They found that combining the modes produced better performance on a whole bunch of measures.…”
Section: Confirmationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some research on collaboration focuses on the benefits of pooling cognitive resources (Kirschner, Paas, & Kirschner, 2009, 2011, since two people have more cognitive resources between them than one person working alone and may also possess complementary knowledge (Johansson, Andersson, & Rönnberg, 2005). Other research suggests that collaboration changes learners' cognitive processes by increasing their use of explanation (Hausmann, Nokes, VanLehn, & van de Sande, 2009;Hausmann, van de Sande, & VanLehn, 2008;Okada & Simon, 1997) and generation of abstract representations (Schwartz, 1995;Shirouzu, Miyake, & Masukawa, 2002). Still other evidence shows that collaboration can improve metacognitive awareness (Larkin, 2006), increase metacognitive behaviors (Whitebread, Bingham, Grau, Pino Pasternak, & Sangster, 2007), and support error monitoring and correction (Brodbeck & Greitemeyer, 2000;Gadgil & Nokes-Malach, 2012;Hall, Dansereau, O'Donnell, & Skaggs, 1989).…”
Section: Collaborative Success and Failurementioning
confidence: 99%