1993
DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1993.tb00391.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sex Roles, Status, and the Need for Social Change

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
(2 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is expected that children with greater stereotype knowledge or rigidity will be more likely than other children to adopt same-gender attributes and to shun other-gender ones. Several studies have found the expected associations (e.g., Aubry et al, 1999; Liben & Bigler, 2002; Miller et al, 2006; Serbin et al, 1993). However, the associations are weak and inconsistent, most likely because this approach fails to satisfy the content match criterion.…”
Section: Three Key Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…It is expected that children with greater stereotype knowledge or rigidity will be more likely than other children to adopt same-gender attributes and to shun other-gender ones. Several studies have found the expected associations (e.g., Aubry et al, 1999; Liben & Bigler, 2002; Miller et al, 2006; Serbin et al, 1993). However, the associations are weak and inconsistent, most likely because this approach fails to satisfy the content match criterion.…”
Section: Three Key Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Nonetheless, they attest to the promise of explicit measures for use with children. Studies have shown (a) that explicit self-perceptions of gender-typed attributes are predictable from explicit measures of gender identity (Fagot, Rodgers, & Leinbach, 2000; Martin & Little, 1990; Martin et al, 2002; Martin, Fabes, Hanish, Leonard, & Dinella, 2006; Ruble et al, 2007) and from explicit measures of gender stereotypes (Aubry, Ruble, & Silverman, 1999; Liben & Bigler, 2002; Martin, Fabes, Evans, & Wyman, 1999; Miller, Trautner, & Ruble, 2006; Serbin, Powlishta, & Gulko, 1993), (b) that explicit gender stereotypes are predictable from explicit self-perceptions (Liben & Bigler, 2002; Martin et al, 1995), and (c) that explicit gender identity is predictable from explicit self-perceptions (Egan & Perry, 2001). All of these effects reflected the operation of cognitive consistency or balance (i.e., gender identity and gender stereotypes predicted children's adoption of same-gender attributes, children projected their own attributes onto the same-sex gender collective, and children used their perceptions of same-gender attributes to estimate their gender typicality).…”
Section: Overview Of the Gender Self-socialization Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Behaving in gender nonconforming ways often has negative consequences for a person’s social relationships (Serbin, Powlishta, & Gulo, 1993). Children expect already at a young age that their peers display sex-typed behavior (behavior viewed as normative for the child’s biological sex); they are more tolerant to gender conforming peers and react more negatively to peers who do not exhibit gender stereotyped behavior, activities, appearance, or traits (e.g., Carver, Egan, & Perry, 2004; Egan & Perry, 2001; Ruble, Martin, & Berenbaum, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%